1/19
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
Visual Word Recognition
The ability to recognise written words through orthographic (spelling), phonological (sound), and semantic (meaning) processing, often without requiring spoken articulation.
Lexical Decision Task
A research method where participants judge whether a string of letters is a real word. Measures recognition speed and lexical access.
Naming Task
Participants read a word aloud as quickly as possible. Measuring speed of phonological retrieval.
Stroop Effect
Interference caused when the meaning of a word conflicts with the task (e.g., saying ink colour of "RED" printed in blue). Demonstrates automatic semantic and phonological access.
Homophone Interference Effect
Mistakenly identifying a homophone as a category member (e.g., “ROWS” mistaken for “ROSE” in a flower task). Phonological processing is automatic.
Masked Phonological Priming
Faster word recognition when preceded by phonologically similar, unconscious primes (e.g., “klip” → “CLIP”).
Word Superiority Effect
Letters are identified more accurately when embedded in real words (e.g., “A” in “TRAP” vs. “MRAU”). Top-down processing (perception guided by prior knowledge) from word-level units enhances letter recognition.
Interactive Activation Model
A connectionist model of reading (McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981) that includes feature, letter, and word levels with top-down and bottom-up activation and lateral inhibition. Explains: Word superiority and contextual effects.
Dual-Route Cascaded (DRC) Model
A model proposing two reading routes: 1. Lexical route (whole word) 2. Nonlexical route (grapheme–phoneme rules)
Applies to: Reading aloud and dyslexia patterns.
Connectionist Triangle Model
A distributed model where reading involves interactive processing among orthography, phonology, and semantics, without distinct routes.
Supports: Gradual skill development and effects of damage (e.g., surface dyslexia).
Surface Dyslexia
Impaired reading of irregular words due to overreliance on grapheme–phoneme conversion.
Example: Reading “PINT” as rhyming with “MINT”.
Phonological Dyslexia
Difficulty reading nonwords and unfamiliar words due to a breakdown in phonological processing.
Example: Trouble pronouncing “VIB” but can read “CAR”.
Deep Dyslexia
Severe dyslexia involving semantic errors, visual errors, and impaired nonword reading.
Example: Reading “TULIP” as “ROSE”.
Orthographic Depth
Degree of consistency between spelling and pronunciation in a language.
Shallow (transparent): e.g., Spanish
Deep (opaque): e.g., English
Saccades
Quick eye movements between fixations during reading (20–30 ms).
Note: No visual information is gathered during saccades.
Fixations
Pauses during which visual information is gathered (~250 ms).
Note: Reading comprehension occurs during fixations.
Parafoveal Preview
The use of peripheral vision to pre-process upcoming words during reading.
Importance: Speeds up processing and informs saccade planning.
Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP)
A method used in speed-reading apps (e.g., Spritz) that presents words one at a time.
Limitation: Hinders comprehension, regressions, and parafoveal preview.
Strong Phonology Hypothesis
The theory that phonological processing is a necessary and mandatory component of reading.
Support: Homophone interference, phonological priming.
Reading Comprehension Difficulty Factors
Text complexity, word predictability, word frequency, and syntactic complexity influence comprehension and eye movement patterns (fixation duration, regressions).