Skills and Expertise Psy2002

0.0(0)
studied byStudied by 0 people
GameKnowt Play
learnLearn
examPractice Test
spaced repetitionSpaced Repetition
heart puzzleMatch
flashcardsFlashcards
Card Sorting

1/21

encourage image

There's no tags or description

Looks like no tags are added yet.

Study Analytics
Name
Mastery
Learn
Test
Matching
Spaced

No study sessions yet.

22 Terms

1
New cards

Stroop task

classic example of a conflict task that reveals how automatic and controlled processes can interfere with each other.

2
New cards

Stroop Task Paradigm

Participants are asked to name the ink colour of a word, which may itself be the name of a different colour (e.g., the word "red" printed in blue ink).

3
New cards

Stroop Task Key finding

Naming the ink colour is slower and less accurate in incongruent conditions due to interference from the automatic process of word reading.

4
New cards

Stroop task Conclusion

Demonstrates a conflict between automatic word reading and controlled colour naming

5
New cards

Stroop task Reveals Multiple dimensions of automaticity

Stimulus-response compatibility

Attentional Focus

Speed of processing

Training and Experience

6
New cards

Stimulus-response compatibility

Response conflict arises when the stimulus format does not match the response format (e.g., saying a colour vs. pointing to it).

Durgin (2000): Found a "reverse Stroop effect" when participants pointed to colour patches, challenging the idea that word reading is always automatic.

7
New cards

Attentional Focus

Besner et al. (1997): When attention is directed to a single letter (e.g., by colouring one letter or using an arrow cue), the Stroop effect can be reduced or eliminated, suggesting that automaticity is not entirely autonomous.

8
New cards

Speed of Processing

Glaser & Glaser (1982): Manipulating stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) showed that word interference is not just due to speed, but also dependent on timing of processing stages.

9
New cards

Training and Experience

MacLeod & Dunbar (1988): With practice, novel associations can become automatic, indicating a continuum rather than an all-or-none model of automaticity.

10
New cards

Automatic and Controlled processes

Often interact hierarchically, especially in skilled performance

11
New cards

Hierarchical Control model (Logan & Crump, 2010)

Skilled behaviours (e.g., typing) involve multiple levels: high-level intentions guide low-level motor routines.

Participants may not be consciously aware of individual actions but can still correct errors, indicating interaction between conscious control and automatic execution.

Evidence: "Illusions of authorship" where feedback manipulated awareness of errors.

12
New cards

Skills vs Habits

Habits are stereotyped, automatic, and inflexible.

Skills involve both automaticity and goal-directed flexibility.

13
New cards

Definition of skill - Pacherie & Mylopoulos

Skilled action is marked by a dynamic interplay between automatic routines and cognitive control, enabling adaptability under varying conditions

14
New cards

Yerkes-Dodson Law

Optimal performance occurs at moderate levels of arousal; too much or too little impairs performance.

15
New cards

Choking Under Pressure (Beilock et al.)

Experts may underperform when overly focused on step-by-step control of well-learned motor tasks (e.g., dribbling with the dominant foot).

16
New cards

Ironic Processing (Wegner 1994)

The harder you try not to do something (e.g., avoid errors), the more likely it is to occur under stress or cognitive load.

17
New cards

Attentional Disruptions (Logan & Crump)

Focusing on automatic components (e.g., individual fingers in typing) can disrupt fluid skilled action.

18
New cards

Deliberate Practice - Ericsson et al. (1993)

Proposed that deliberate practice—effortful, structured, and feedback-driven—is the primary factor in expert performance.

19
New cards

Ericsson - key limitations

Other factors matter

Not all practice is equally effective

Empirical challenges

20
New cards

Other factors matter

Innate ability, environmental opportunities, and motivation also play crucial roles.

Not everyone has equal access to training or coaching (e.g., support, time, resources).

21
New cards

Not all practice equally effective

Some evidence (e.g., Macnamara & Maitra, 2019) suggests that deliberate practice explains only part of the variance in expertise.

22
New cards

Ericsson empircal challenges

Some replications found weaker correlations between practice hours and performance than originally reported.

Elite performers sometimes engage in less practice than predicted but still outperform others, suggesting other influences.