1/11
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
Diagnosis and Classification- gender bias (limitation)
P: A significant issue in the diagnosis of schizophrenia (Sz) is gender bias.
E: Longenecker et al. (2010) found that men are diagnosed with Sz significantly more often than women, a trend that has become more pronounced since the 1980s. This may be due to men’s higher genetic vulnerability to the condition. However, another explanation is that gender bias in diagnosis might play a role, with women often being overlooked for Sz diagnosis due to their higher functioning. For example, women are more likely to maintain jobs and good family relationships, as noted by Cotton et al. (2009), which may make their symptoms less obvious and lead to underdiagnosis.
C: This suggests that the gender bias in diagnosis could be skewing the prevalence rates of Sz, potentially leading to an underestimation of the condition in women.
H: However, gender bias may be a contributing factor to misdiagnosis, which could result in an over-representation of men with Sz while women’s symptoms are downplayed. This raises questions about the accuracy and fairness of diagnostic practices.
I/D: Socially sensitive - This issue is socially sensitive because it can lead to real-world consequences, such as women being denied appropriate treatment due to underdiagnosis, while men may be overdiagnosed and unnecessarily stigmatised. It also reinforces harmful gender stereotypes — for example, the belief that men are more mentally unstable or that women are emotionally stronger and better at coping. These assumptions can affect clinical judgment and contribute to unequal access to mental health care.
Diagnosis and Classification - cultural bias (limitation)
P: A significant issue in the diagnosis of schizophrenia (Sz) is cultural bias.
E: Studies have shown that Black British and Black Americans are more likely to be diagnosed with schizophrenia than white individuals, despite similar rates of the condition in African and West Indian populations. This suggests that the over-diagnosis may not be due to genetic vulnerability but rather cultural factors. For example, positive symptoms of Sz, such as hearing voices, might be more culturally accepted in certain cultures (e.g., communication with ancestors) but may be misinterpreted as irrational or bizarre by psychiatrists from different cultural backgrounds. Escobar (2012) found that white psychiatrists may misinterpret symptoms and distrust Black patients, leading to inaccurate diagnoses.
C: This indicates that cultural biases in diagnostic practices may contribute to the over-diagnosis of schizophrenia in Black populations, questioning the validity of diagnostic criteria across different cultural contexts.
H: However, higher diagnosis rates in Black populations may reflect genuine increased risk due to environmental factors like urban living, social isolation, and economic disadvantage — all linked to schizophrenia. This suggests the overrepresentation may not result from cultural bias but from greater exposure to stressors that increase vulnerability.
I/D: Nature vs Nurture - higher diagnosis rates in Black populations may result from environmental factors such as social disadvantage and stress (nurture) rather than solely biological causes (nature). This highlights the need to consider both genetic and environmental influences when understanding schizophrenia.
Biological Explanations - strong evidence supporting the role of genetic susceptibility in the development of schizophrenia (strength)
P: There is strong evidence supporting the role of genetic susceptibility in the development of schizophrenia (Sz).
E: Tienari et al. (2004) conducted an adoption study in Finland, comparing the rates of schizophrenia in adopted children whose biological mothers had the condition with those in a control group with no genetic risk. They found that schizophrenia was more common in the genetically at-risk group, even though both groups were raised in similar adoptive environments. This provides compelling support for a genetic basis to Sz, as the increased risk was present despite being raised apart from the biological family, indicating that environmental factors alone could not account for the condition.
C: This body of evidence strongly supports the idea that genetic factors significantly contribute to the risk of developing schizophrenia, suggesting that genetic susceptibility plays a critical role in the onset of Sz.
H: However, adoption itself can be a significant source of early life stress, such as separation from biological parents and identity disruption. These stressors may interact with genetic vulnerability, contributing to the development of schizophrenia. This makes it difficult to isolate genes as the sole cause, suggesting that environmental factors linked to adoption may partly explain the increased rates seen in adoptees.
I/D: Nature vs Nurture – While genetic factors play a significant role in schizophrenia susceptibility, the evidence also suggests that environmental influences cannot be ignored in the development of Sz. This highlights the need for an integrated approach that considers both genetic and environmental factors.
Biological Explanations - evidence for dopamine hypothesis is mized and doesn’t povide a complete explanation (limitation)
P: The evidence for the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia is mixed and does not provide a complete explanation.
E: While dopamine has been strongly linked to schizophrenia, recent research has shown that other neurotransmitters also play a role. For instance, the study by Ripke et al. (2014) identified genes related to the production of neurotransmitters other than dopamine. This suggests that while dopamine may be important, other neurotransmitters are likely involved as well. Further supporting this, research has increasingly focused on the role of glutamate in the development of schizophrenia, with Moghaddam & Javitt (2012) suggesting that disruptions in glutamate signaling could also be a key factor.
C: This highlights the complexity of the biological basis of schizophrenia and suggests that dopamine alone cannot fully explain the disorder, as other neurotransmitters like glutamate also appear to play a significant role.
H: However, there is some evidence for the dopamine hypothesis - Curran et al (2004) found that dopamine antagonists, like amphetamines, that increase the levels of dopamine, make Sz worse and can produce Sz-like symptoms in non-sufferers.
I/D: Reductionism vs Holism – While the dopamine hypothesis provides valuable insights into the biological mechanisms of schizophrenia, it may be limited by its reductionist approach. A more holistic perspective, which considers genetic, environmental, and neurochemical factors, may be necessary to fully understand the complexity of schizophrenia.
Psychological Explanations - Research support for family dysfunction as a risk factor (strength)
P – Research support for a family dysfunction as a risk factor
E- Read et al. (2005) reviewed 46 studies of child abuse and Sz and concluded that 69% of adult women in-patients with a diagnosis of Sz had a history of physical abuse, sexual abuse, or both in childhood. For men, the figure was 59%. Adults with insecure attachments to their primary caregiver are also more likely to have Sz (Berry et al., 2008).
C – Evidence supports family dysfunction as a risk factor.
H – However, most of this evidence was gathered after the development of symptoms, and Sz may have distorted patients’ recall of childhood experiences. This creates a serious problem of validity. A much smaller number of studies have been carried out prospectively to see if childhood experiences predicted adult characteristics, with mixed results from prospective studies.
I/D – Reductionism vs Holism: The explanation of Sz as solely linked to family dysfunction can be considered reductionist because it oversimplifies the complex condition to just one factor. This overlooks other biological, genetic, and environmental factors that contribute to Sz. A holistic approach, incorporating multiple influences, would provide a more comprehensive understanding of the disorder.
Psychological explanations - Research support for dysfunctional information processing (strength)
P – Research support for dysfunctional information processing
E- Stirling et al. (2006) compared 30 patients with a diagnosis of Sz with 18 non-patient controls on a range of cognitive tasks including the Stroop test. In line with Frith’s theories of central control dysfunction, patients took twice as long to name the ink colours as the control group.
C – The findings suggest that information processing is impaired in those with Sz, supporting dysfunctional information processing.
H – However, while there is a large body of evidence linking information processing impairments with Sz, cognitive theories do not explain the origins of these faulty cognitions or Sz itself. They only account for the proximal causes (current symptoms), leaving the distal causes (origins of the condition) unaddressed.
I/d – Nature vs Nurture: This research focuses on the cognitive impairments associated with Sz, but it doesn’t address the potential role of environmental factors in shaping these cognitive processes. While cognitive theories emphasize internal, individual factors, understanding Sz fully requires considering how external experiences or upbringing may influence these cognitive deficits.
Biological Therapies - Research evidence supports the effectiveness of both typical and atypical antipsychotics in treating schizophrenia (strength)
P – Research evidence supports the effectiveness of both typical and atypical antipsychotics in treating schizophrenia (Sz).
E – Thornley et al. (2003) found that Chlorpromazine led to better overall functioning and reduced symptom severity compared to a placebo, and reduced relapse rates. Meltzer (2012) found that Clozapine is more effective than both typical antipsychotics and other atypicals in 30–50% of treatment-resistant cases.
C – This suggests that antipsychotic medication can be an effective treatment for managing Sz symptoms, particularly in cases where other treatments have failed.
H – However, the variation in response to different drugs, and inconclusive findings when comparing Clozapine to drugs like Risperidone, indicate that treatment is not universally effective and may depend on individual differences.
I/d – Nomothetic vs Idiographic: Much of the drug research aims to find generalisable treatments (nomothetic approach), but the mixed responses suggest that a more idiographic, personalised approach may be necessary to tailor treatment to individual needs.
Biological Therapies - Typical antipsychotics are associated with a range of side effects that can significantly affect a patient’s quality of life (limitation)
P – Typical antipsychotics are associated with a range of side effects that can significantly affect a patient’s quality of life.
E – These include dizziness, agitation, sleepiness, weight gain, and tardive dyskinesia (involuntary facial movements). The most serious side effect is neuroleptic malignant syndrome (NMS), which can be fatal and results in high temperature, delirium, and coma. Clozapine also has serious side effects, such as agranulocytosis, which requires regular blood tests.
C – This demonstrates that the side effects of antipsychotic medications can be debilitating, potentially outweighing the benefits for some patients.
H – However, for individuals with severe schizophrenia and extremely poor quality of life, the benefits of antipsychotic medication may outweigh the risks. Without treatment, patients may face frequent relapses, loss of functioning, or even suicide. In such cases, side effects, while serious, may be considered an acceptable trade-off for symptom control and stability.
I/D – Ethical Implications: Antipsychotics pose ethical concerns due to their serious side effects. However, informed consent is crucial—patients must be fully aware of the risks and voluntarily agree to treatment. This respects their autonomy and helps ensure that even when the trade-offs are significant, the decision remains ethically sound.
Psychological Therapies - Psychological treatments show some evidence of effectiveness, but this is limited (strength/limitation)
P – Psychological treatments for schizophrenia, such as CBT and family therapy, show some evidence of effectiveness but have limitations.
E – Jauhar et al. (2014) found that CBT has a significant but small effect on both positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Pharoah et al. concluded that family therapy can reduce hospital readmissions and improve quality of life for patients and families, but noted inconsistencies and low-quality evidence in some studies.
C – This suggests that while psychological treatments can offer some benefits, the overall effectiveness is limited, and schizophrenia remains difficult to treat.
H – However, it could be argued that psychological treatments are still valuable when used in combination with medication, as they help address specific symptoms and improve coping strategies.
I/d – Nomothetic vs. Ideographic: The debate here is whether psychological treatments for schizophrenia should focus on general principles (nomothetic) that apply to all patients or more individualised, person-centered approaches (ideographic) that consider the specific needs and circumstances of each patient.
Psychological Therapies - They aim to improve quality of life, but do not cure SZ (limitation)
P – Psychological treatments for schizophrenia aim to improve quality of life but do not cure the disorder.
E – CBT helps patients manage and sometimes challenge symptoms, family therapy reduces familial stress, and token economies encourage socially acceptable behavior. However, these therapies cannot eliminate the disorder or prevent all symptoms, only reduce their impact. Consequently, patients often require long-term, ongoing treatment, which can be costly and demanding for both patients and healthcare systems. The inability to cure schizophrenia highlights the complexity of the disorder and the need for continued research into more effective interventions.
C – While psychological treatments improve daily functioning and symptom management, they cannot be seen as cures, highlighting a major limitation in their effectiveness.
H – However, psychological treatments are still vital because they help patients develop coping skills, reduce distress from symptoms, and improve relationships with family and peers. These improvements can lower relapse rates and hospital admissions, making psychological therapies an important complement to medication, even though they don’t cure schizophrenia.
I/D – Holism vs reductionism. Psychological therapies for schizophrenia adopt a holistic approach by addressing multiple aspects of a patient’s life, including emotional wellbeing, social relationships, and coping strategies. This contrasts with reductionist approaches that focus narrowly on symptoms or biological causes. Holism highlights the importance of treating the whole person and improving overall quality of life, which is crucial given that these therapies do not cure schizophrenia but support better daily functioning.
Interactionist Approach - Support for the effectiveness of combined treatment (strength)
P – Support for the effectiveness of combined treatment
E – Studies like Tarrier et al. (2004) show the benefits of combining treatments. They found that patients receiving a combination of medication and CBT, or medication and supportive counseling, had lower symptom levels than those who received medication only, although there was no difference in readmission rates.
C – This suggests that combining treatments can lead to better symptom management and more holistic care.
H – However, the lack of difference in hospital readmission rates indicates that while combined treatments can improve symptoms, they may not address all aspects of the condition or lead to more long-term improvements.
I/d – Reductionism vs. Holism: The findings highlight a debate over whether treatment for schizophrenia should focus solely on biological or psychological factors (reductionist approach), or whether an interactionist, more holistic approach (which combines both) offers a more effective long-term solution.
Interactionist Approach - The treatment-causation fallacy - therpaies may not actually treat Sz (limitation)
P – The treatment-causation fallacy in the interactionist approach
E – Turkington et al. argue that the interactionist approach is logical because combining biological and psychological treatments is more effective than either approach alone. However, the fact that combined treatments work better doesn’t necessarily prove that schizophrenia is caused by both biological and psychological factors.
C – This suggests that while combination treatments can be effective, it does not provide conclusive evidence for the interactionist model of schizophrenia’s causation.
H – However, the fact that combined treatments yield better outcomes could indicate that schizophrenia may involve both biological and psychological factors in different ways. While it doesn’t confirm causality, it suggests that addressing both areas could be essential for effective treatment. (And it still helps improve quality of life and reduces symptoms - does it matter then?)
I/d – Nature vs. Nurture: This highlights the debate over whether schizophrenia is caused by biological factors, psychological factors, or a combination of both, and whether combining treatments is an effective way to address both types of causation.