1/27
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
Should you waive your rights?
NEVER - police interrogate people they already believe are guilty
The Interrogation
Create a desire to escape
Place a suspect in small, bare, windowless room
Begin by making a confident accusation of guilt
No legal time limit; possibility of sleep deprivation
Suspects are in highly vulnerable psychological states
Tactics for increasing psychological vulnerability
Repeated, unending, cycle of accusations
Suggestions regarding leniency
Maximization
Instill hopelessness, imply harsh punishment absent confession
“You will be much better off if you talk to us”
Minimization
Provide a way out, minimize seriousness of offense
“I can understand why you would’ve been upset”
Factors that Increase Vulnerability of a Suspect
Youth of suspect
Low intelligence of suspect
Lack of a clear memory of the event in question
Key Tactic Often Used by Interrogators
Can lie or “bluff” about evidence
Including claims of false evidence
Types of false confessions
Voluntary
Compliant
Internalized
Compliant Confession
Confessing to merely get out of the interrogation
Internalized Confession
After long hours and coercive lies from police, people begin to believe that they have committed the crime
Kassin & Kiechel (1996) - Computer Crash Paradigm
Reaction time task (press “ALT” key and computer crashes
Manipulated vulnerability and false evidence
Computer always crashes (do they confess?)
No false evidence - 35% confessed (slow pace) and 65% (fast pace)
With false evidence - 89% confessed (slow pace) and 100% (fast pace)
Russano et al., 2005 - Cheating Paradigm
Some are induced to “cheat” while others are not
Both true and false confessions, unlike computer crash paradigm
Implying or directly promising leniency increases true and false confessions
Comports with concepts of minimization/maximization
Perillo & Kassin, 2011 - Corroboration Inflation
Evidence “bluffs” increase false confessions via belief in future exoneration
Indications that confessions impact other types of evidence
Hasel & Kassin (2009) – eyewitnesses
Kukucka & Kassin (2012) – handwriting
Kassin et al (2013) – fingerprint and other forensic “science”
When does a confession have a greater impact?
A confession has a greater impact on verdicts than eyewitness or character statements (Kassin & Neumann, 1997)
85% of false confession cases that went to trial resulted in conviction (Drizin & Leo, 2004)
How does recording help the situation?
Police: fewer coercive tactics, improved diagnosis of guilt
Juries: better at determining guilt with whole transcript
Police reports sometimes omit important details
Suspects: None
Being aware of camera didn’t affect likelihood of confession or conviction
Confirmation Bias
The tendency to seek out, select, and/or interpret information in ways that support existing beliefs
Ignore or discount evidence that disconfirms
What can motivate confirmation bias?
The desire to:
be accurate
reach a specific conclusion
Forensic Confirmation Bias
The class of effects through which an individual’s preexisting beliefs, expectations, motives, and situational context influence the collection, perception, and interpretation of evidence during the course of a criminal case
Evidence & Confirmation Bias
Belief in suspect’s guilt influences judgements of handwriting evidence
Belief in suspect’s guilt influences judgement of inconclusive polygraphs
“Linear” vs. “Circular” Investigations
Determine suitability of sample prior to any knowledge/exposure to comparison
Allow reassessment only with firm justification and documentation
Blind Testing
Do not allow for knowledge about relevant case
Use an evidence lineup
Less suggestive
Reduce effect of contextual information
Can figure out error rate
Step 1 of a Plea
Defense attorney (if the defendant has one) and prosecutor meet, at which time either attorney (usually prosecutor) suggests a plea bargain
Step 2 of a Plea
Defendant is informed of the plea bargain. If the defendant agrees to the terms of the prosecutor’s offer, s/he proceeds to a plea colloquy
Step 3 of a Plea
Judge makes sure the defendant understands all agreements and waiver of rights (waived rights include: right to a jury trial, right to confront accusers, etc.)
“Shadow of the trial” model - Redich et al., 2017
One should plead guilty if offered sentence is equal to or less than the expected value of the trial (Trial sentence x Potential conviction)
Example:
Probability of conviction at trial = .8
Sentence if convicted at trial = 10 years
Expected value of trial = 8 years
Therefore, defendant should accept a plea offer with a sentence of 8 years or less
Trial Penalty Model - Redich et al., 2017
Prosecutors, defenders, and judges form an interdependent workgroup with shared goals such as disposing of cases efficiently and minimizing uncertainty – to this end, the workgroup established norms or going rates for defendants willing to plead guilty
Defendants who reject these going rates are penalized
Descriptive Theory of Doorholding
P(Doorhold) = Cd + Ct + E - D/Vt - B
P (probability)
Cd (characteristics of door holder)
Ct (characteristics of target)
E (eye contact)
D (distance)
Vt (Velocity of target)
B (# of bystanders)
Innocence Problem
Sentencing guidelines (e.g. mandatory minimums) may allow for prosecutors to compel false guilty pleas (Dervan, 2012)
Research for Innocence Problem
Edkings & Dervan (2012)
Plea Bargaining Simulation Study (Zimmerman & Hunter, 2018)
Wilford, Zimmerman, Yan, and Sutherland (2021)