1/171
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
Developmental psychology
The field of study that explores patterns of stability, continuity, growth and change that occur throughout a person’s life.
Domains of development
Physical - The growth of the body and its organs, the functioning of physiological systems including the brain, physical signs of ageing, changes in motor abilities etc.
Cognitive - Changes and continuities in perception, language, learning, memory, problem solving and other mental processes.
Psychosocial - Changes and continuities in personal and interpersonal aspects such as motives, emotion, personality traits, interpersonal skills, relationships and roles played in the family and in society.
Nature vs nurture
Nature: biogenetic and hereditary influences
Nurture: environment influences (relationships, culture)
Maturation vs experience
Maturation: developmental changes that occur over time (due to biology) regardless of experiences
Experience: developmental changes related to specific experiences
Continuity vs discontinuity
Continuity: gradual development, with change happening in increments over time.
E.g., at age 2 years a child cries when their mother leaves a room, but by age 25 years they can go a week without a phone call.
Discontinuity: development occurs in distinct stages or steps, with each stage bringing different behaviours and qualities than previous stages.
E.g., butterfly to a caterpillar.
Active vs passive
Active: individuals are active agents in their own development, seeking out opportunities to grow, learn, and master increasingly difficult tasks.
Passive: development occurs through events in the environment that require individuals to respond, leading to changes in behaviour.
Universal vs specific - development experiences
Universal: a general trend/pattern of development that is applicable to all individuals and groups.
Specific: patterns of development are specific to a particular context or setting (e.g., individual experiences or cultural circumstances)
Frued’s personality structures
People are driven by motives and emotional conflicts of which they are largely unaware
People’s lives are shaped by their earliest experiences
Three personality structures:
Id: unconscious, selfish instincts and biological needs and desires (pleasure principle)
Ego: reality and problem solving, learning how to redirect desire for instant satisfaction to realistic pursuits (reality principle)
Superego: conscience, and sense of right and wrong
Erikson's psychological stages of development
Proposes that dialectical conflicts are the mechanisms for growth
Emphasis on social influences, such as peers, family, school etc.
Emphasis on rational and active resolution of conflicts
People emerge from each crisis/conflict “with an increased sense of inner unity, with an increase of good judgement, and an increase in the capacity to do well”
Bronfenbrenner (1917-2005)
Views development as a process of reciprocal, patterned interactions between the individual and their physical and social environment.
Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological systems theory proposed four interacting and overlapping contextual levels:
Microsystem: Face-to-face interactions
Mesosystem: Connections between microsystems
Exosystem: Indirect influences
Macrosystem: General aspects of society
Chronosystem – encompasses time and the critical life and historical events that also influence development.
This model was later amended to the bioecological systems theory to encompass the individual and their biology and physical growth.
Classical conditioning - Ivan Pavlov (1849 -1936)
Unconditioned stimulus/unconditioned response
Neutral stimulus paired with unconditioned stimulus
Neutral stimulus --> Conditioned stimulus
Conditioned stimulus/unconditioned response
Reflex learning
Operant Conditioning - B. F. Skinner (1904 - 1991)
Reinforcement strengthens response
Punishment weakens response
Positive: addition
Negative: withdrawal
Social Learning Theory - Albert Bandura (1925- 2021)
Learn through modelling: observing others doing something
Then imitate the modelled behaviour
Can observe the rewards or consequences associated with behaviours
Can also learn not to do something (e.g., child watching others falling in the playground might deter them from doing the same thing)
Cognitive Development - Jean Piaget (1896 - 1980)
Children actively construct new understandings of the world based on their experiences
Four discrete stages of thought
Sensorimotor operation stage
Pre-operational stage
Concrete operational stage
Formal operational stage
Assimilation: fitting new information into existing cognitive structures or schema.
Accommodation: when assimilation doesn’t work, new schemas are formed.
Adaptation: when existing schemas are deepened or strengthened through processes of assimilation or accommodation.
Vygotsky's zone of proximal development
Cultural nature of human development
Culture as a tool ‘within’ a person
Social interaction drives cognitive development
Scaffolding: the framework of support and assistance provided by others (e.g., teachers,
parents, peers)
With this scaffolding, support can be gradually reduced
Bronfrenbrenners bioecological systems theory
Erikson’s psychosocial stages of development
Frueds stages of psychosexual development
Operant conditioning
Theory vs research
Theory
A ‘theoretical’ framework aiming to explain human development
Incorporates many ‘assumptions’
Help guide research and practice.
Micro-theory: narrow scope
Macro-theory: encompass large fields of development.
Research
• Collect data to test and refine theories and hypotheses (predictions)
• Derive concrete observations/findings.
• Help guide future research, practice, and policy.
Methods of developmental research
Case study, naturalist observation, experiments, self-report assessments
Case study
In-depth focus on one individual, group, or event
Aim is to fully understand that experience by compiling a complete picture
Naturalistic Observations
Observing the normal habits of an individual/group in a natural setting
Goal is to limit the researcher intrusion
Increases the real-life validity, but is open to subjectivity and interpretation
Often require two observers to check the accuracy and objectivity of the collected data
Experiments
• Aim is to test causal hypotheses by systematically manipulating one variable at a time
• Impose a tight level of control over the testing environment and any factors that could influence behaviour
• However, these controls could mean findings aren’t generalisable (applicable) to the real world
Self-report Assessment
Individuals report on their own experience or perspectives
Often via interviews or surveys
Allows us to get an insight into functioning or experiences that are difficult to observe (e.g., personality)
Open to reporting bias; requires a level of insight into behaviours; relies on recall
For self-report data in particular, a multi-informant approach is often the best, including from child, caregivers, teachers etc.
Assessments
Assessment with examples
Include a standardised assessment of functioning or performance
Establishes a standardised and objective approach
Quality of test relies on the quality of the items and whether they work equally well for all individuals (diverse backgrounds)
Examples
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)
• National Assessment Program – Literacy and Numeracy (NAPLAN)
• Birth weight
• Mental State Examination
Correlation studies
Looks at the relationship between two or more variables, but doesn’t manipulate these variables, doesn’t permit causal inferences but sometimes is the only way to research something. Cross-sectional, longitudinal & cross-sequential.
Cross-sectional study
Studies a group of participants at one point in time.
Advantages:
Can examine differences between participants
Can examine differences according to age OR explore one particular age/stage in-depth
Cost-effective and time efficient
Disadvantages:
Can’t distinguish between age effects (variation in experiences due to chronological age) and cohort effects (social and historical changes affecting a specific group with a shared event)
Can explore associations between variables (e.g., personality and mental health) but only at one time.
Difficult to infer cause and effect
Longitudinal study
Studies the same group of participants over multiple time-points.
Advantages:
Study developmental trajectories over time
Can explore how early experiences (predictors) relate to later development (outcomes)
Disadvantages:
Expensive (time and resource intensive)
Attrition (drop-out over time)
Cross-sequential study
Combines cross-sectional and longitudinal research. Multiple groups followed across multiple time points.
Allows investigation of development among individuals over time.
Allows investigation of differences in development between groups of different ages.
What is Infancy
A period of rapid growth and development in a range of areas:
Physical, Perceptual, Cognitive, Language, Social and Emotional
Infant Motor Development- Newborn Reflexes
Reflexes are unlearned, involuntary responses to stimuli
Survival reflexes are adaptive. E.g. breathing, eye-blink, sucking
Primitive reflexes are less adaptive and typically disappear in early infancy. E.g. Babinski reflex, grasping reflex
Motor skill types & how they’re developed
Motor development follows two trends:
Cephalocaudal - pattern of development in humans that occurs from the head down to the feet.
Proximodistal - growth pattern in which development radiates from the center of the body outward.
Gross motor skills - Movement of large muscles of arms, legs, and torso
Fine motor skills - Movement of small muscles such as fingers, toes
Habituation
The process of learning to be bored with a stimulus
After repeated presentation with the same visual stimulus, the infant becomes bored and looks away
If a different stimulus is presented and the infant regains interest, researchers conclude that the infant has discriminated between the two stimuli
Preferential looking
A method used to assess infant perception by presenting two visual stimuli and measuring which one the infant looks at longer. If an infant consistently looks at one stimulus more, it suggests they can perceive a difference and have a preference.
Evoked potentials
Researchers can assess how an infant’s brain responds to stimulation by measuring its electrical conductivity
Infant - Operant conditioning
Infants can learn to respond to a stimulus (to suck faster or slower or to turn the head) if they are reinforced for the response
Infants: Vision
At birth, infants have vision, but lack acuity
Can see more clearly about 20 – 25cm
Objects at 6 metres as distinct as objects at 180 metres for adults
Improves steadily during infancy
Attracted to patterns that have light-dark transitions, or contour
Attracted to displays that are dynamic rather than static
Young infants prefer to look at whatever they can see well
Around 2 or 3 months, a breakthrough begins to occur in the perception of forms
Depth perception in infants
Gibson and Walk (1960): Classic study to examine depth perception in infants using the visual cliff
Infants can perceive the cliff by 2 months (tend to be curious rather than fearful)
Hearing
Basic capacities are present at birth
Can hear better than they can see
Can localise sounds
Can be startled by loud noises
Can turn toward soft sounds
Prefer relatively complex auditory stimuli
Can discriminate among sounds that differ in loudness, duration, direction, and frequency/pitch
Early development
Sensory experience is vital in determining the organisation of the developing brain
The visual system requires stimulation early in life to develop normally
Early visual deficits (i.e., congenital cataracts) can affect later visual perception
Exposure to auditory stimulation early in life affects the architecture of the developing brain and influences auditory perception skills
Infant Cognition- Piaget’s Sensorimotor Stage
The world is understood through the senses and actions
The dominant cognitive structures are the behavioural schemes that develop through coordination of sensory information and motor responses
Substages of the Sensorimotor Stage
Reflexes: (first month)
Reflexive reaction to internal and external stimulation
Primary circular reactions: (1-4 months)
Infants repeat actions relating to their own bodies
Secondary circular reactions: (4-8 months)
Repetitive actions involving something in the infant’s external environment
Coordination of secondary schemes: (8-12 months)
Secondary actions are coordinated in order to achieve simple goals (i.e., pushing or grasping)
Tertiary circular reactions: (12-18 months)
Experimentation; actions are repeated with variations
Beginning of thought : (18 months)
Symbolic thought permits mental representation, imitation, and recall
The Development of Object Permanence
Object permanence develops during the sensorimotor period
From 4-8 months, “out of sight, out of mind”
By 8-12 months, make the A-not-B error
By 1 year, A-not-B error is overcome, but continued trouble with invisible displacement
By 18 months, object permanence is mastered
Research suggests that infants may develop at least some understanding of object permanence far earlier than Piaget believed.
By 3 months, infants appear to understand that objects have qualities that should permit them to be visible when nothing obstructs them
Emotions
Earliest emotion – crying
Hunger, anger, pain, fussiness
Other emotions
Joy and laughter, 3 - 4 months
Wariness, 3 - 4 months
Surprise, 4 months
Fear, 5 - 8 months
More complex emotions in toddlerhood
Infants: Self
Infants develop an implicit sense of self through their perceptions of their bodies and actions
In the first 2 or 3 months, infants discover they can cause things to happen
After 6 months, infants realise they and other people are separate beings with different perspectives, ones that can be shared
Around 18 months, infants recognise themselves visually as distinct individuals - Lewis and Brooks-Gunn (1979): Mirror test
Attachment
A strong and enduring emotional bond that develops between an infant and a caregiver during the infant’s first years of life
Characterised by reciprocal affection and a shared desire to maintain physical and emotional closeness
Psychoanalytic - I love you because you feed me
Learning - I love you because you are reinforcing
Cognitive - I love you because I know you
Ethological - I love you because I was born to love
Key figures in attachment theory:
John Bowlby (1907-1990)
Mary Ainsworth (1913-1999)
Piaget’s Theory & Core Concepts
Children actively construct new understandings of the world based on their experiences
Schemes are mental structures or frameworks that help children organize and interpret information (e.g., a baby’s grasping scheme for holding objects).
Assimilation is when a child fits new information into existing schemes (e.g., calling all four-legged animals "dog").
Accommodation is when a child changes or creates new schemes to fit new information (e.g., learning that a cat is different from a dog and adjusting their understanding).
Piaget’s Sensorimotor Stage
Infant - 2
The world is understood through the senses and actions
The dominant cognitive structures are the behavioural schemes that develop through coordination of sensory information and motor responses
Goal-directed behaviour/intentionality
Symbolic thought/representations
Infants move from understanding the world through senses and actions toward understanding through symbolic thought
Become capable of mental representations
Mental imagery - Internal representation of an external event
Language - Not just a communication system, but a means of representing objects and events in an abstract way
Symbolic play – Pretending
Play
The first pretend play emerges early in the second year – Play in which one actor, object, or action symbolises or stands for another
In the earliest pretend play, the infant performs actions that symbolise familiar activities such as eating, sleeping, and washing
Between the ages of 2 and 5, pretend play increases in frequency and in sophistication
Inverted U shape: nonexistent around 15 months, peaks around 5-7 years before decreasing
Social pretend play is universal - The content of play is influenced by culture
Social pretend play emerges around 3 – 4 years, or earlier in the context of a more proficient partner such as an older sibling, mother, or father.
Cognitive skills developed from pretend play
Social referencing
Using another person’s response to an ambiguous situation as a guide for one’s own response
Decentration
Reading intentionality in others
The Development of Intentionality
Seeing oneself (and others) as intentional agents is one of the most basic elements of cognition in social contexts
Intentionality underpins more complex social cognition such as Theory of Mind
Piaget’s Pre-operational Stage
2-7
Symbolic representations and capacity:
Language
Pretend play
• Can include imaginary companions
Can refer to the past and future
Object permanence
A focus on perceptual salience – the most obvious features of an object or a situation – means that preschoolers can be fooled by appearance
They may also have difficulty with tasks that require logic
Reliance on perceptions and lack of logical thought means that children have difficulty with conservation - the idea that certain properties of an object or substance do not change when its appearance is altered in a superficial way
Cognitive limitations of pre-operational stage
Centration -Focusing on one aspect of a problem or object
Irreversible thought - Cannot mentally undo an action
Static thought - Focusing on the end state rather than the changes that transform one state into another
Difficulty with classification - Using criteria to sort objects on the basis of characteristics such as shape, color, function
Lack class inclusion, the ability to relate the whole class (furry animals) to its subclasses (dogs, cats)
Egocentrism
Theory of Mind
The ability to attribute mental states— beliefs, intents, desires, pretending, knowledge—to oneself and others and to understand that others have beliefs, desires and intentions that are different from one's own.
Develops around age 4–5 in children
Crucial for empathy, social interaction, and predicting others’ behavior
Early development of social cognition in infants
Intentionality: Infants recognize that others act with purpose (e.g., reaching for a toy on purpose, not by accident).
Perspective-taking: Begins in infancy and develops over time; understanding that others can have different views or knowledge.
Joint attention: Around 9 months, infants share focus with others by following gaze or pointing.
By 12 months: Infants point to inform others (not just to request).
By 18 months: Infants distinguish between intentional and accidental actions.
Piaget’s Concrete Operations Stage
7-11
In middle childhood, children move from preoperational to concrete operational stage
Non-conserving, transitional, conserving
Demonstrate the ability to perform operations
Mental actions on concrete situations/objects
Decentration
Can focus on two or more dimensions of a problem at once
Reversibility of thought
Can mentally reverse or undo an action
Transformational thought
Can understand the process of change from one state to another
In this stage we see a shift from understanding being driven by perceptual salience to logical reasoning
Seriation
The ability to arrange items mentally along a quantifiable dimension such as weight or height
Transitivity
The understanding of relationships among elements in a series
Less egocentrism
Classification abilities improve
Can classify objects by multiple dimensions and can grasp class inclusion
Piaget’s Formal Operations Stage: Adolescents
This takes place gradually over years
Formal operations are mental actions on ideas
They permit systematic and scientific thinking about problems, hypothetical ideas, and abstract concepts
Piaget’s pendulum task
The postformal stage
Some researchers have postulated a fifth stage of cognitive development- the postformal stage (e.g., Commons and Bresette, 2006)
In postformal thinking, decisions are made based on situations and circumstances, and logic is integrated with emotion as adults develop principles that depend on contexts.
Limitations of Piaget’s Theory
The timing of stage progressions is more variable than Piaget proposed.
Piaget was more likely describing children’s performance (what a child usually does) rather than their competence (the limits of their abilities).
Piaget may have over-estimated some abilities:
Adults frequently fail to use formal operations in daily life.
Formal operations can be domain-specific (successfully applied in some contexts but not to others).
Cognitive-developmental approach: Lev Vygotsky (1896-1934)
Key Idea: Cognitive development is shaped by social interaction and cultural tools (e.g. language).
Learning happens first socially, then individually (internalised).
Introduced the concept of the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD)
Morality
Morality is a sense of behavioural conduct that differentiates intentions, decisions, and actions between those that are good (or right) and bad (or wrong).
Three Components of Morality
Affective (Emotional)
Feelings around right/wrong (guilt, concern for others).
Emotions motivate moral thoughts/actions = moral affect.
Negative emotions (e.g., shame, guilt) deter bad behaviour.
Positive emotions (e.g., pride) reinforce doing good.
Empathy (feeling others' emotions) is key to moral dev.
Empathy motivates prosocial behaviour (helping, sharing, concern for others' welfare).
Cognitive
How we understand right/wrong and decide how to act = moral reasoning.
Behavioural
What we actually do in moral situations (e.g., resisting cheating, helping others) = moral behaviour.
Moral Reasoning
Cognitive developmental theorists study morality by looking at the development of moral reasoning – the thinking process involved in deciding whether an act is right or wrong
Moral reasoning is believed to progress through an invariant sequence – a fixed and universal order of stages, each of which represents a consistent way of thinking about moral issues that is different from the stage preceding or following it (Kohlberg’s Stages of Moral Development)
Piaget’s theory of moral development
Includes three aspects:
Premoral period
During the preschool years, children show little awareness or understanding of rules and cannot be considered moral beings
Heteronomous morality
Children 6 to 10 years old take rules seriously, believing that they are handed down by parents and other authority figures and are sacred and unalterable
They judge rule violations as wrong based on the extent of damage done, not paying much attention to whether the violator had good or bad intentions
Autonomous morality
At age 10 or 11, most children enter a final stage of moral development in which they begin to appreciate that rules are agreements between individuals – agreements that can be changed through a consensus of those individuals
In judging actions, they pay more attention to whether the person’s intentions were good or bad than to the consequences of the act
Moral Reasoning- Kohlberg
Kohlberg proposed that moral reasoning develops through a series of stages, grouped into three levels. He believed that people progress through these stages as they mature and gain life experience.
Pre-conventional (kids):
Stage 1: Avoid punishment
Stage 2: Self-interest
Conventional (teens/adults):
Stage 3: Gain approval
Stage 4: Follow rules/laws
Post-conventional (some adults):
Stage 5: Social contract/fairness
Stage 6: Universal ethics/conscience
Criticisms of Kohlberg’s theory
Scoring procedures not sufficiently objective or consistent
Content of dilemmas too narrow
Dilemmas not aligned with real-life
No distinction between moral knowledge and social conventions
Gender and culture bias
Moral behaviour according to social-learning theory
Moral behaviour is learned in the same way that other social behaviours are learned: through observational learning and reinforcement and punishment principles
Social-learning theorists believe moral behaviour is believed to be strongly influenced by the situation
Due to situational influences, what we do (moral performance) is not always reflective of our internalised values and standards (moral competence)
Attachment Theory
Foundation of the Theory:
Attachment theory is based on the idea that humans have a biological predisposition to form emotional bonds, particularly with caregivers, for survival and well-being.
John Bowlby, a British psychiatrist and psychoanalyst who developed the foundational concepts of attachment theory.
Bowlby's theory draws on evolutionary psychology, suggesting that attachment behaviors, like crying for comfort or seeking proximity, are adaptive mechanisms that ensure the survival of infants.
Early Relationships Matter:
The quality of these early relationships, characterized by responsiveness, consistency, and sensitivity, significantly impacts a child's development and their ability to form healthy
relationships later in life.
Attachment Styles & influence on adult relationships
Attachment theory identifies different attachment styles, including secure, anxious-preoccupied, dismissive-avoidant, and fearful-avoidant, which are thought to emerge from early experiences with caregivers.
Mary Ainsworth is a developmental psychologist who collaborated with Bowlby and developed the "Strange Situation" method, a tool for assessing attachment styles in infants.
Influence on Adult Relationships:
Attachment styles formed in childhood can influence patterns of relating in romantic relationships, friendships, and other significant connections throughout adulthood.
The Impact of Historical Trauma on Attachment
The Stolen Generations and its impact on familial bonds and attachment.
Intergenerational trauma and its effects on attachment behaviours.
Parental trauma is linked to detachment in the next generation, where individuals tend to distance themselves from close relationships (Spiel & Bornstein, 2023)
Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) in parents, particularly maltreatment, are associated with behavioral problems in children, emphasising the need for interventions that address parental trauma to break the cycle of intergenerational trauma (Wang et al., 2022).
Maladaptive Coping Mechanism
Misdiagnosis weaponised against Aboriginal peoples
Mistrust in systems and services resulting in disengagement
Cultural Limitations of Attachment Theory
Attachment theory in Australian child welfare systems misrepresents Aboriginal families and children's needs, perpetuating inequities.
use of non-Indigenous constructs in child protection interventions reflects dominant cultural perceptions, causing harm to Aboriginal communities.
the mother-infant dyad, does not fully capture the communal and extended family structures prevalent in Aboriginal communities.
This theory often misrepresents Aboriginal parenting practices, leading to inappropriate child welfare interventions
The reliance on attachment theory in legal decisions concerning Indigenous children often overlooks the importance of cultural continuity and community-based caregiving systems
Protective Factors for Aboriginal Attachment
Kinship = extended family + community
➤ Core to identity and protective care for children
Cultural attachment is protective
➤ Strong community ties reduce stigma, support healthy choices
Traditional practices support development
➤ Collective child-rearing and Elders strengthen family function and resilience
Place attachment matters
➤ Connection to land (e.g., Cherbourg) supports identity, social & cognitive growth
The Promotion of Social and Emotional Wellbeing (SEWB)
A holistic concept that recognises connection to land, culture, spirituality, ancestry, family and community are as important for wellbeing as physical health. This concept also acknowledges the impact of past events and policies on the SEWB of individuals today.
Psychosocial development
the development of the personality, and the acquisition of social attitudes and skills, from infancy through maturity.
Key points of Mary Ainsworth Attachment Theory
It is the reunion behaviour that is the most telling
Lack of observable distress does not mean lack of underlying anxiety e.g. avoidant attachment
Formation of Internal Working Models (IWMs)
Internal Working Models are mental representations of ourselves, others, and relationships, formed through early attachment experiences (Bowlby, 1969).
Based on interactions with primary caregivers (especially in infancy). Eg, If a caregiver is responsive and consistent, the child forms a positive IWM (e.g., "I am lovable, others are trustworthy").
IWMs guide future relationships, emotional regulation, and expectations.
They’re mostly unconscious but can affect how we relate to others throughout life.
Two dimensions of PARENTING STYLES
Acceptance-responsiveness
refers to the extent to which parents are supportive, sensitive to their children’s needs, and willing to provide affection and praise when their children meet their expectations
Demandingness-control (sometimes called permissiveness- restrictiveness)
refers to how much control over decisions lies with the parent rather than with the child
Authoritarian parenting and outcomes
High demandingness-control and low acceptance-responsiveness
Parents impose many rules, expect strict obedience, rarely explain why the child should comply with rules, and often rely on power tactics such as physical punishment to gain compliance
Children of authoritarian parents tended to be moody and seemingly unhappy, easily annoyed, relatively aimless, and unpleasant to be around
Authoritative parenting and outcomes
high demandingness-control and high acceptance- responsiveness
parents set clear rules and consistently enforce them, but they also explain the rationales for their rules and restrictions, are responsive to their children’s needs and points of view, and involve their children in family decision-making
Children of authoritative parents were the best adjusted – cheerful, socially responsible, self-reliant, achievement oriented, and cooperative with adults and peers
Permissive parenting and outcomes
High in acceptance-responsiveness but low in demandingness-control
Permissive parents are indulgent with few rules and few demands
They encourage children to express their feelings and impulses and rarely exert control over their behavior
Children of permissive parents were often impulsive, aggressive, self-centered, rebellious, aimless, and low in independence and achievement
Uninvolved (Neglectful) parenting and outcomes
Low demandingness-control and low acceptance-responsiveness
They seem not to care much about their children and may even reject them
Uninvolved parents may be so overwhelmed by their own problems that they cannot devote sufficient energy to expressing love and setting and enforcing rules
Children of neglectful parents display behavioral problems such as aggression and frequent temper tantrums as early as age 3
They tend to become hostile and antisocial adolescents who abuse alcohol and drugs and get in trouble
Benefits of Only Children
Sometimes stereotyped as self-centred or spoilt, however, this is not borne out in research.
Research suggests higher in:
Self-esteem
Positive personality
Achievement motivation
Academic success
Saracho & Spodek (1998) definition of play
Intrinsically, not extrinsically motivated
Process-, not product- oriented
Creative and non-literal
Having implicit rules
Spontaneous and self-initiated
Free from major emotional distress
RELATIONSHIPS WITH PEERS
A peer is a social equal, someone who functions at a similar level of behavioral complexity, often someone of similar age
Peer relationships have developmental value
Peers help children learn that relationships are reciprocal
Peers force children to hone their social perspective-taking skills
Peers contribute to social-cognitive and moral development in ways that parents cannot
Contact with peers comes simultaneously with cognitive development including:
Major advances in language development
Major advances in perspective-taking abilities, and hence capacity for cooperative play, prosocial behaviour (and antisocial behaviour!) increases
Advances in problem-solving ability means improved capacity to tackle conflict
Sociometric measures & social status categories
Researchers study peer-group acceptance through sociometric techniques
Methods for determining who is liked and who is disliked in a group
Using sociometric techniques, children may be classified into the following categories of social status (Coie & Dodge, 1988):
Popular – well liked by most and rarely disliked
Rejected – rarely liked and often disliked
Neglected – neither liked nor disliked (isolated children who seem to be invisible to their classmates)
Controversial – liked by many but also disliked by many (the fun-loving child with leadership skills who also bullies peers and starts fights)
Average – in the middle on both the liked and disliked scales
GENDER-ROLE DEVELOPMENT
Process of learning gender-consistent behaviours.
By age 2, children recognize typical gender behaviours and start gender labelling themselves and others but don’t yet understand gender stability.
Preschoolers (4-7 years) show strong rigidity about gender stereotypes.
Rigidity decreases in primary school as gender identity firms up and thinking becomes more flexible.
Children may exaggerate gender roles to understand them better (Maccoby, 1998).
Non-conforming children may face social consequences.
The nervous system
Not a static network of interconnected elements; rather, it is a plastic (changeable), living organ that grows and changes continuously in response to its genetic programs and its interactions with the environment.
Neurons
The basic functional units of the nervous system. They take in information from other neurons (reception), integrate those signals (conduction), and pass signals to other neurons (transmission).
Glial cells
Nourish, protect, and physically support neurons and are thought to be particularly critical in brain development. One type of glial cell, the oligodendrocyte, covers the axons of neurons with myelin, a substance critical to the effective functioning of the brain.
Speed of propagation of the action potential is determined by?
diameter of axon (bigger = faster)
presence or absence of a myelin sheath
Cortical changes
Infancy and early childhood is characterised by a dramatic period of synaptogenesis, following by an adaptive process of cell death and pruning. There is another notable surge of synapse growth just before puberty.
The strengthening or elimination of synapses is dependent on environmental demands or experience; those that are more often used are strengthened and those that are rarely used are eliminated.
Overall, grey matter (neuronal cell bodies, dendrites, and glial cells) development follows an inverted-U pattern of growth, first thickening in volume, peaking, and then thinning
White matter changes
White matter increases in a roughly linear pattern throughout childhood, adolescence, and into early adulthood
However, different brain structures myelinate at different times: myelogenetic cycles
Sensory/motor pathways myelinate early
Regions mediating higher-order functions myelinate late (e.g. the prefrontal cortex)
Development of the Prefrontal Cortex
Grey matter volume: inverted U (early increases followed by gradual decreases starting in late childhood and continuing into adulthood)
White matter volume: Myelination- thought to be complete in the early 20s
Therefore, there is relatively ‘late’ maturation of the prefrontal cortex
Functions of the Prefrontal Cortex
‘Executive’ functions
Working memory
Cognitive flexibility
Inhibitory control
Reasoning
Problem solving
Planning
Executive attention
Executive Functions in children
EF includes:
Working memory: paying attention, remembering facts, using info to complete tasks.
Inhibitory control: following rules, managing emotions, delaying gratification.
Cognitive flexibility: planning, judging, self-correcting.
EF helps children regulate behaviour and develop social, emotional, and cognitive skills over time.
Moffitt et al. (2011): Poor inhibitory control in childhood predicts worse adult outcomes (health, income, crime, happiness).
Explicit Memory
involves intentional recollection of previous experiences (conscious, accessed directly)
Four conclusions about development of explicit memory:
Older children are faster information processors; maturation of nervous system leads to improved short-term memory capacity and efficiency but age does not impact sensory register or long-term memory capacity
Older children use more effective memory strategies in encoding and retrieving information
Older children know more about memory
Older children know more in general and larger knowledge base improves ability to learn and remember
Implicit Memory
apparent when retention is exhibited on a task that does not require intentional remembering. (unconscious, accessed indirectly)
From a developmental point of view, a variety of studies show that explicit memory is significantly increasing throughout infancy and childhood, where implicit doesn’t change as much
STM capacity (Dempster, 1981)
2 years old: 2 items
5 years old: 4 items
7 years old: 5 items
9 years old: 6 items
Metamemory
your awareness and understanding of your own memory processes. It's a key part of metacognition (thinking about thinking).