Looks like no one added any tags here yet for you.
purpose of family
promote survival
provide emotional and financial support
socialization
socialization
process through which children acquire values, standards, knowledge, behaviours seen as appropriate for their role in culture
benefits of parents
closeness to mother at age 16 = marital satisfaction at age 32
low father involvement at age 7 and 11 = low exam performance at age 16 and criminal records at 21
parenting training/interventions = children show reduction in behavioural problems and delinquency
aspects of parenting
warmth/support/acceptance/responsiveness and control/demandingness dimensions
permissive parent, authoritative parent, uninvolved parent, authoritarian parent
discipline: reinforcement/punishment, power assertion, inductive discipline
permissive parent
highly supportive but makes few rules and trusts rather than monitors - also called indulgent
high support and low control dimensions
authoritative parent
highly supportive and closely monitors and sets rules
high support and control dimensions
uninvolved parent
sets few rules, does not monitor, offers little active support
little support and little control dimensions
authoritarian parent
sets many rules and closely monitors but offers little support
high control and low support dimensions
parent relations
conflict = poorer outcomes for kids
divorce associated with increased problems in kids
why does divorce cause problems in children?
parent conflict
stress
diminished parenting
economic difficulties/changes
social difficulties/changes
absence of parent/fear of absence
relocation
same sex/gender parents
no difference in children’s adjustment, personality, achievement, sexual orientation
report feeling different/subjected to social slights - still feel positive about their families
parenting quality is what is important, not gender or sexuality
sibling relationships
shaped by parents and parent-child relationships - typically neither vertical (hierarchy) or horizontal (equal) power structures
marked by warmth/support and conflict
non-voluntary
shaped by age, age gaps, gender, biological vs. adopted vs. step siblings vs. half-siblings
caregiver sibling relationship
one sibling serves as a quasi-parent for the other
buddy sibling relationship
both siblings like each other and try to be like each other
casual/uninvolved sibling relationship
siblings have little to do with each other
critical/conflictual/rival relationship
one sibling tries to dominate the other - teasing, fighting
functions of sibling relationships
practice communication and social skills
buffer for peer rejection, parent conflict, stressful experiences
try out new behaviours
opportunity for learning about gender
promote individuality
only children
siblings are not necessary for healthy development
only children tend to have higher self-esteem, do better in school
no differences in china - more normative to be an only child? more contact with cousins?
grandparents
useful for survival - boost emotional well-being, serve as buffers in children growing up in risky contexts
different roles: influential, supportive, passive, authority-oriented (least common), detached (most common)
culture and parenting
use of warmth/support and control in all cultures - warmth/support appears beneficial for development
across cultures: differences in how warmth/support and control are expressed, different “ideal” of control
what is valued as good parenting differs
there is a lot of variability within cultures
Chinese parents
less warmth? → tend to withhold praise, believed to lead to self-satisfied children
more controlling? → belief in deeply-involved parents, respect for family/authority
some studies show no negative effects of higher-control parenting in Chinese families, but more recent studies show similar negative effects of high-control in Chinese and North American families
Latino parents
more warmth? → familismo: desire for family ties, for family support
more controlling? → respeto: fulfill obligations, maintain harmonious relationships
some studies find positive outcomes linked with warmth + hostile control
cultural normativeness
not all parents within a culture think and behave the same - parents’ practices not congruent with others in their cultural context adaptive for children?
culture and siblings
which is seen as primary bond - parent-child or sibling
amount of time spent together
caregiving roles
culture and extended family
large cultural and ethnic differences in proportion of children living with extended families - black families more likely to live with larger extended family (helps with child-rearing, reduce costs, transmission of culture)
friendships
close, mutual, reciprocal, voluntary relationship
friends as toddlers
live nearby, have nice toys, like to play
rewards vs. costs
friends as kids
shared interests, take care of/support each other
friends as adolescents
shared interests and values, engage in self disclosure and intimacy
from developed perspective taking
function of friendships
emotional and physical support
buffer in stressful times
development of social skills
social comparison
stimulation
conflict and resolution
model and reinforce behaviour
cliques
smaller, voluntary, friendship-based groups
shared interests and attitudes
tend to have shared background - initially same gender move to mixed gender, initially mixed race move to same race
50 to 75% of teens are part of one
crowds
reputation-based groups, less voluntary
function of cliques
same benefits of friendships and social groups
sense of belongingness
for straight teens, can support/guide interest in romantic relationships
function of crowds
locate individuals within social environment
contribute to sense of identity and self-concept
establish social norms
peer status
usually measured through sociometric status - how liked/dislike someone as by their peers
most children receive average ratings (30-60%)
popular/likeable peer status
skilled at initiating and maintaining positive interactions
good at recognizing and regulating emotions
good at perspective taking
rated by teachers as cooperative, friendly, helpful, leaders
assertive, not pushy
not always the same as perceived popularity
rejected peer status
rejected-aggressive: instrumental and relational aggression, physical aggression, bullying (externalizing problems)
rejected-withdrawn: poor social skills, socially anxious, difficulty with social goals (internalizing problems)
rejected-aggressive/withdrawn (academic problems)
controversial peer status
share characteristics of both popular and rejected children → can be helpful/cooperative, but can also be disruptive/aggressive
less stable status, can change
neglected peer status
timid, shy, lack of social skills - often not bothered by classification
less stable status, can change
average peer status
most common - moderately sociable, average cognitive skills
what leads to having a certain peer status?
social skills/social behaviour
temperament
interpretations → hostile attribution bias, rejection sensitivity
parents
physical attractiveness
name (?), race and ethnicity (?)
perceived popularity
mix of positive and negative traits
social skills
aggression: instrumental, relational
physical attractiveness
more variable, based on changing norms
think Regina in Mean Girls
cultural variability in likability, perceived popularity, admiration (Zhang et al., 2019)
between China (collectivist) and US (individualistic) - students asked to give who they like the most, think are the most popular, and admire the most
prosocial behaviour and academic achievement linked with likability and perceived popularity across both cultures - more linked for Chinese adolescents
positive behaviour less predictive of perceived popularity in US
cultural values may impact peer status
dating
tends to begin ~14-15 years
early adolescence: dating for status
middle/late adolescence: dating for kindness, honesty, intelligence
influences on romantic relationships
peer relationships
family relationships
family factors (older siblings, single parents, family instability)
culture
media? sexual orientation?
functions of romantic relationships
establish autonomy
develop intimacy
sense of belonging
feelings of self-worth
status
furthering development of gender/sexual identity
starting romantic relationships
early starters = atypical sequence, associated with negative outcomes (externalizing behaviours)
late bloomers = link to delayed social development, lower self-esteem
impact of romantic relationships
Positive: more social acceptance, social competence
Negative: more risk-taking behaviour
outcomes vary based on culture, timing, characteristics of romantic partners, relationship quality
potential negative components of relatonships
breaking up (most common trigger for depressive episode)
dating violence (depression, suicidal ideation, drug use, teen pregnancy, dropping out of school
online peer interactions
distinct from face to face?
increased anonymity
different social cues
different emphasis on physical appearance
more public, long-lasting
easier to find similar others
all-day access to friends
more quantifiable
online social interactions and academic/emotional adjustment to university (Mikami et al., 2019)
UBC jumpstart students monitored for their facebook use during their first week of university
good: greater connection with Facebook friends = fewer psychopathology symptoms
bad: facebook friends’ deviant posts = lower gpa, verbal aggression = less attachment to university (stronger for students with low face-to-face social acceptance)
online friendships can serve some of the same functions as in-person
piaget’s theory of morality
<4 years: premoral reasoning
4-7 years: heteronomous morality
>11/12 years: autonomous morality (morality of cooperation)
evolves with cognitive development and time with peers
premoral reasoning (piaget)
less than 4 years old - lack of moral compass
heteronomous morality (piaget)
rules from authority (parents)
focus on consequences
moral realism, immanent justice
4-7 year
moral realism
things are taken at face value - moral claims report facts and are true
immanent justice
rules are fixed and punishment automatically follows bad behaviour
autonomous morality (piaget)
rules as social agreement
sensitive to fairness and equality
focus on motives and intentions
moral relativism
>11/12 years
moral relativism
moral judgments are relative to specific culture or individual, there are no universal moral truths
support for piaget’s theory of moral development
moral reasoning correlated with performance on tests of cognitive development and children do increasingly consider intentions and motives
underestimated children: kids younger than 4 think about morality and young children do consider intentions
Kohlberg’s theory of moral development
moral dilemmas - focus on children’s reasoning, not on specific answers
Level 1: pre-conventional moral reasoning
Level 2: conventional moral reasoning
Level 3: post-conventional moral reasoning
level 1: pre-conventional moral reasoning (Kohlberg)
Stage 1: punishment and obedience orientation
right = obeying authority, avoiding punishment
Stage 2: instrumental and exchange orientation
right = what will result in rewards - tit for tat
Level 2: conventional moral reasoning (Kohlberg)
Stage 3: mutual interpersonal expectations, relationships, and interpersonal conformity orientation
right = what is expected by people/society
importance of being good, social acceptance
Stage 4: social system and conscience orientation
right = doing one’s duty, adhering to authority, upholding social order
law and order
level 3: post-conventional moral reasoning
Stage 5: social contract of individual rights orientation
right = uphold rules in best interest of group, universal values of life/liberty
emphasis on law, but willingness to change with rational considerations
Stage 6: universal ethical principles (now combined with stage 5)
right = self-chosen ethical principles that reflect universal principles of justice
civil disobedience
support for Kohlberg’s theory
not maturation, not directly taught - based on advances in cognition (own thinking, discussions with peers) and individual differences/influences of family, school, peers
criticisms: no focus on gender or culture (differences in moral principles and moral development)
antisocial behaviour
aggression, cheating
instrumental aggression
motivated by a desire to obtain a goal
reactive aggression
in response to other’s behaviour
physical aggression
intent to physically harm
high in toddlerhood and early childhood
verbal aggression
threats, name-calling, yelling
social/relational aggression
directed towards damaging reputation/relationships
increases in middle childhood/adolescence (more for girls)
cheating
many forms - is common in youth (80-90% in high school)
appears lower in childhood, rapid increase in adolescence
why do kids and teens cheat?
pressure for performance, high grades
social comparison
not enough time to prepare/study
lack of interest
perceive teacher to be unfair or uncaring
cheating influences
situation: more common when not being monitored
peers: more common when observe/perceive others to be cheating
mindset: more common for fixed mindset about intelligence
praise: more common when told you are “smart” (maintain ability) - children more likely to cheat after hearing classmate is smart
prosocial behaviour
actions intended to help or benefit other people
motivation behind is altruism
altruistic motives
altruistic motives
helping others not for external rewards or social approval
nature vs nurture of prosocial behaviour
nature: heritable genetic traits that strongly contribute
young children exhibit altruism ~14 months old
nurture: socialization important
development of prosocial behaviour
rapid increases in toddler and preschooler period
similar overtone of selfishness at 2
self-interest reduces 3-5 years
early self-interest less pronounced in collectivist cultures
socialization
process through which we learn norms, values, behaviours, social skills essential for competent participation in society
culture is the backdrop
family/parents are starting point + base for interaction
children are active agents - caregivers adapt actions for each child
cultural differences in prosocial behaviours
distinction between collectivist and individualistic cultures is simplistic, obscures intracultural variability
children in collectivist cultures are more altruistic - cultural emphasis on prioritizing well-being of community
Asian children showed more passive sharing, American children showed more elicited sharing
prosocial value emphasis between cultures
collectivistic societies: emphasize obedience and conformity
individualistic societies: emphasize child autonomy and independence
basis of prosocial motivation between cultures
India: altruistic acts viewed as moral obligations
US: norms of equality may play stronger role
parental effects on prosocial behaviour
children more prosocial when they have formed secure attachments with their parents and have close relationships with siblings
profile of parents with prosocial kids
authoritative
avoid using harsh punishments/strict rules
warm and enjoy shared activities
model, encourage, explain why prosocial behaviour is expected
parenting practices related to prosocial behaviour in kids
parental warmth
positive socialization fostering emotional competence
offering material rewards for prosocial behaviour decreases later pro-sociality
encouraging through praise and encouraging participation in household chores may be effective in fostering prosocial behaviours
cultural differences in parental prosocial instruction
Delhi mothers
emphasized hierarchical relatedness
provided more opportunities to help at home, material rewards
used punitive practices
children more likely to help
German mothers
used praise more frequently
expected prosocial behaviour is be intrinsic
cross-cultural similarities and differences in parenting (Lansford, 2022)
behaviour that promotes infants’ survival is similar across cultures
belief systems, physical environment difference, beliefs about what child is capable of drive cultural variation in parenting
cognitive stimulation (education, playing, reading, outside)
warmth (physical affection vs. acts of service)
control and monitoring (level of involvement)
discipline (normality of different discipline forms)
children more likely to cheat after overhearing another classmate is smart (Zhao et al., 2018)
68% of 5 year olds cheated on a guessing game when overhearing adults talk about how smart a previous participant was, 3 years old did not have the same significant relationship
praise is a powerful motivator for behaviour and shaper of moral development
cultural differences in development
self-concept (relational/situational terms vs. internal/abstract terms)
self-esteem (contributing to groups vs. individual accomplishments)
parenting warmth and control
scaffolding (physical/gestural vs. verbal)
intelligence mindset (fixed vs. dynamic)
cultural similarities in development
gender differences in aggression
theory of mind/false belief tasks
parenting (protection and nurturing)