14.2 Prosocial Behavior

0.0(0)
studied byStudied by 0 people
0.0(0)
linked notesView linked note
full-widthCall with Kai
GameKnowt Play
New
learnLearn
examPractice Test
spaced repetitionSpaced Repetition
heart puzzleMatch
flashcardsFlashcards
Card Sorting

1/21

encourage image

There's no tags or description

Looks like no tags are added yet.

Study Analytics
Name
Mastery
Learn
Test
Matching
Spaced

No study sessions yet.

22 Terms

1
New cards

prosocial behavior

voluntary behavior intended to benefit another, such as helping, sharing with, and comforting others — rooted in the capacity to feel empathy and sympathy

2
New cards

empathy

an emotional response to another’s emotional state or condition that reflects the other person’s state or condition

3
New cards

sympathy

feeling of concern for another in response to the other’s emotional state or condition

4
New cards

age of empathy and sympathy development

children can feel empathy and sympathy by the second year of life

5
New cards

14 months

become emotionally distressed when they see other people who are upset and express verbal/nonverbal concern for adult who has been hurt

6
New cards

18-25 months

  • toddler in lab studies sometimes share a personal object with an adult whom they have seen being harmed by another

  • more likely to try to comfort someone who is upset then become upset themselves, indicating that they know who it is that is suffering

7
New cards

2-4 years

  • some types of prosocial behaviours increase, while others decrease

  • 3-4 years olds are much more likely than 2 year olds to provide assistance/verbal reassurance

8
New cards

perspective-taking

the ability to understand that others have different thoughts and feelings, which is necessary for expressing empathy and sympathy

9
New cards

cooperation

Working together with another child or adult to reach a goal that benefits them both, which can be driven by sympathy or a child’s sense of fairness

10
New cards

biological factors

  • proposed humans a biologically predisposed to be prosocial

  • people who help others are more likely then less helpful people to be assisted when they themselves are in need and, thus, are more likely to survive and reproduce

  • pertain to the human species as a whole and do not explain individual differences in empathy, sympathy and prosocial behavior

11
New cards

genetics factors do contribute to individual differences in these characteristics

  • twins’ reports of their own empathy and prosocial behavior are considerably more similar for identical then fraternal twins

  • role of genetic factors in children’s prosocial concerns for others and in their prosocial behavior increases with age

  • identifies specific genes that might contribute to individual differences in prosocial tendencies

12
New cards

how else might genetic factors affect empathy, sympathy and prosocial behaviour?

  • differences in temperament — differences in ability to regulate emotions are related to their empathy and sympathy

  • regulation is also related to children’s theory of minds and theory of mind predicts children’s prosocial behavior

  • the effect of heredity on sympathy and prosocial behavior might involve individual differences in social cognition as well as temperament

13
New cards

socialization of prosocial behavior

the process by which parents and the environment teach children about prosocial values and behaviors

14
New cards

three ways parents socialize prosocial behavior

  1. modeling and teaching prosocial behavior

  2. arranging opportunities for children to engage in prosocial behavior

  3. disciplining children and eliciting prosocial behavior from them

    • also communicate and reinforce cultural belief about the value of prosocial behavior

15
New cards

modeling behavior

children tend to imitate the helping and sharing behavior of others, especially adults with whom they have a positive relationship

16
New cards

what does modeling behaviour help explain

help explain the fact that parents and children tend to be similar in their levels of prosocial behavior, although heredity may also contribute to the similarly between parent and child in sympathy and helpfulness

17
New cards

opportunities for prosocial activities

providing opportunities to engage in helpful activities can increase their willingness to take on prosocial tasks at a later time and foster empathy and cooperation

18
New cards

discipline and parenting style

the way parents discipline their children can influence the development of empathy and prosocial behavior, particularly through reasoning and understanding moral values

19
New cards

constructive parenting

parenting style that is supportive and constructive, associated with higher levels of prosocial behavior in children

20
New cards

peer influences

relationships with peers that help children learn and practice moral principles that translate into prosocial behavior

21
New cards

developmental stages of prosocial behavior

prosocial behaviors increase from ages 2 to 4, with notable changes in how children respond to others' distress

22
New cards

interventions

  • research does not follow firm conclusions about cause-and-effect relations

  • some school interventions have been effective at promoting prosocial behavior in children, so environmental factors must contributes to its development

  • the research underlying such interventions indicates that experience in helping and cooperating with others, exposure to prosocial values and behaviours, and adults’ use of reasoning in discipline contribute to the development of prosocial behavior