Looks like no one added any tags here yet for you.
prosocial behavior
voluntary behavior intended to benefit another, such as helping, sharing with, and comforting others — rooted in the capacity to feel empathy and sympathy
empathy
an emotional response to another’s emotional state or condition that reflects the other person’s state or condition
sympathy
feeling of concern for another in response to the other’s emotional state or condition
age of empathy and sympathy development
children can feel empathy and sympathy by the second year of life
14 months
become emotionally distressed when they see other people who are upset and express verbal/nonverbal concern for adult who has been hurt
18-25 months
toddler in lab studies sometimes share a personal object with an adult whom they have seen being harmed by another
more likely to try to comfort someone who is upset then become upset themselves, indicating that they know who it is that is suffering
2-4 years
some types of prosocial behaviours increase, while others decrease
3-4 years olds are much more likely than 2 year olds to provide assistance/verbal reassurance
perspective-taking
the ability to understand that others have different thoughts and feelings, which is necessary for expressing empathy and sympathy
cooperation
Working together with another child or adult to reach a goal that benefits them both, which can be driven by sympathy or a child’s sense of fairness
biological factors
proposed humans a biologically predisposed to be prosocial
people who help others are more likely then less helpful people to be assisted when they themselves are in need and, thus, are more likely to survive and reproduce
pertain to the human species as a whole and do not explain individual differences in empathy, sympathy and prosocial behavior
genetics factors do contribute to individual differences in these characteristics
twins’ reports of their own empathy and prosocial behavior are considerably more similar for identical then fraternal twins
role of genetic factors in children’s prosocial concerns for others and in their prosocial behavior increases with age
identifies specific genes that might contribute to individual differences in prosocial tendencies
how else might genetic factors affect empathy, sympathy and prosocial behaviour?
differences in temperament — differences in ability to regulate emotions are related to their empathy and sympathy
regulation is also related to children’s theory of minds and theory of mind predicts children’s prosocial behavior
the effect of heredity on sympathy and prosocial behavior might involve individual differences in social cognition as well as temperament
socialization of prosocial behavior
the process by which parents and the environment teach children about prosocial values and behaviors
three ways parents socialize prosocial behavior
modeling and teaching prosocial behavior
arranging opportunities for children to engage in prosocial behavior
disciplining children and eliciting prosocial behavior from them
also communicate and reinforce cultural belief about the value of prosocial behavior
modeling behavior
children tend to imitate the helping and sharing behavior of others, especially adults with whom they have a positive relationship
what does modeling behaviour help explain
help explain the fact that parents and children tend to be similar in their levels of prosocial behavior, although heredity may also contribute to the similarly between parent and child in sympathy and helpfulness
opportunities for prosocial activities
providing opportunities to engage in helpful activities can increase their willingness to take on prosocial tasks at a later time and foster empathy and cooperation
discipline and parenting style
the way parents discipline their children can influence the development of empathy and prosocial behavior, particularly through reasoning and understanding moral values
constructive parenting
parenting style that is supportive and constructive, associated with higher levels of prosocial behavior in children
peer influences
relationships with peers that help children learn and practice moral principles that translate into prosocial behavior
developmental stages of prosocial behavior
prosocial behaviors increase from ages 2 to 4, with notable changes in how children respond to others' distress
interventions
research does not follow firm conclusions about cause-and-effect relations
some school interventions have been effective at promoting prosocial behavior in children, so environmental factors must contributes to its development
the research underlying such interventions indicates that experience in helping and cooperating with others, exposure to prosocial values and behaviours, and adults’ use of reasoning in discipline contribute to the development of prosocial behavior