1/56
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
Yin (1969) Exp 1
found a robust inversion effect for faces that was larger than that for other sets of stimuli supporting the specificity account of face recognition mechanisms
Diamond & Carey (1986)
found a robust inversion effect for dog images as that for faces (when observes were experts dog breeders) supporting the expertise account of face recognition mechanisms
Parr & Heintz (2006) Exp1
The inversion effect suggests that chimps like humans, show face configural processing
Parr & Heintz (2006) Exp2
shows significant impairments when faces were manipulated to disrupt second-order relations, both the split feature trials and the split plus rearranged feature trials.
Parr & Heintz (2006) Exp3
pixelating faces using a large radius filter, affecting both first- and second-order relations, affects subjects’ recognition performance
Gauthier et al., (1999)
Similar FFA activation for faces and Greebles for Greeble experts supporting the expertise account of face recognition mechanisms
Michel et al (2006)
Golby et al (2001)
showed superior recognition memory for same-race compared to other-race faces
Golby et al (2001)
revealed that compared to other-race faces, same-race faces were associated with greater activation in the FFA previously identified as areas of initial specialization for the perception of faces.
FFA was more active for same- race than for other-race faces in at least 84% of participants.
Vizioli et al (2010)
Western Caucasian & East Asian observers were more accurate at recognizing same-race compared to other-race faces
This was indexed by the larger inversion effect recorded for same vs other race faces.
Vizioli et al (2010)
The amplitude of the face inversion effect was largest for own-race vs other-race faces.
The reduce behavioural and N170 inversion effect for other-race faces could be due to reduce expertise at scrutinizing configural information.
Gervais et al (2011) maths scores
being objectified damages women’s maths score`
after being objectified men increase maths score
Gervais et al (2011) gaze affecting women’s body
Objectifying gaze affected women’s body self-perception indexed by measures of body surveillance, body shame, and body dissatisfaction
Avenanti et al (2005) exp1
Decreased muscle activation (i.e., lower MEPs) in response to observing pain compared to non-painful or neutral conditions.
Reed et al (2003)
Inversion disrupts our ability to exploit configural information
Bernard et al (2012)
Both male & female showed a reduced inversion effect for sexualized women → perceiving them as object-like / featural processing
Both male & female P showed a robust inversion effect for sexualized men perceiving them as face/body-like / configural processing
Berard et al (2015) Exp1
replicated results of 2012 study
larger inversion effect for men
small effect for women = processed like objects
Berard et al (2015) Exp2a
Pixelating sexual body parts eliminated difference in configural processing between sexualized female & male bodies.
P showed a normal inversion effect (harder to recognize inverted images) for both women and men.
No difference in recognition performance between sexualized female and male bodies after pixelation
Berard et al (2015) Exp2B
pixelating reduces objectification
Bernard et al (2015) Exp 3
humanising allowed for inversion effect preving objectification
both men & women show reduced IE in response to sexualised images of women
Bernard et al (2018)
larger n170 amplitude for
female bodies compared to male
sexualsied bodies compared to non
inverted images compared to upright
lack of inversion effect for sexualised bodies suggest reduced configural processing
women more likely to be portrayed as sexual & when non sexualised, are less likely to be processes holsitically
Bernard et al (2019) Exp1
skin exposure does not necessarily cause objectification;
bodies were processed holistically whether they had a lot of skin showing or not.
Bernard et al (2019) Exp2
Posture, not amount of skin exposure, is the key driver of cognitive objectification.
Suggestive postures lead to treating bodies more like objects (less holistic/configural processing).
Bernard et al (2019) Exp3
Bodies in suggestive postures are processed less configurally and are more objectified
this effect is driven by the suggestiveness of the posture, not by differences in body shape or asymmetry
Kteily et al et al (2015)’s Exp1
Arabs & muslims were rated as signfic less evolved than other groups
american & europeans were rated significantly more evolved
Kteily et al et al (2015)’s Exp1a
uses personality & empathy test
doctors & americans seen as more evolved than mexican immigrants, welfare recipients & muslims
Kersbergen & Robinsons (2019) Exp1
obese americans were considered less evolved than americans
Kersbergen & Robinsons (2019) Exp2
Us citizen condition 39% of p donated
for obese us charity 16%
Kersbergen & Robinsons (2019) Exp3
obese individuals were dehumanised
indians had larger dehumanisation
more likely to reduce animal suffering
Boysen et al (2020) Exp1
mental illness is dehumanised
over ½ SD separated rationing of p w/MI from americans
less dehumanised than violent criminals but more than americans
Boysen et al (2020) Exp3
blatant dehumanisation most prevalent for violent criminals then ppl w/MI & americans
Mi is highly stigmatised
Bruneau et al (2018)
Low-status groups are dehumanised similarly to animals, with specific brain regions (IPC, IFC, dMPC) showing no distinction between animals and dehumanised humans.
IFC helps drive this dehumanisation.
Sellaro et al (2015)
congruent trial = faster RT due to alignment w/attuitude
incongruent trial = slower RT suggesting automatic associations even w/out explicit prejudice
Anodal stimulation significantly reduced implicit bias
reduction of D-IAT scores suggests MPFC is recruited to control for implicit bias
Civile et al (2019) Exp1
stronger FIE for regular faces
weaker inversion effect for autistic faces
labelling faces as autistic results in configural processing
Civile et al (2019) Exp2
showed larger inversion effect after humanising info about faces labelled as autistic
Makumel et al (2010)
hand cued p to perfrom full hand grasp
p smiled or formed depending on cue
suggest existence of multiple systems w/neural
Avenanti et al (2005) exp1
decreased motor excitability during observation of needle in model’s hand
Inhibitory sensorimotor response → decreased muscle activation in response to observing pain
Higher MEP amplitude w/Q-tip on hand
Avenanti et al (2005) exp2
no significant modulation of MEP amplitude recorded from FDI or ADM muscles when observers viewed foot stimulations
watching pain stimulation didn’t elecit MEPs
showing specific activation
Avenanti et al (2005) exp3
results were consistent w/selectivity in ½
MEP recorded from ADM muscle during needled = decreased muscle activation
anatomically specific pain response → doesn’t affect MEPs in unrelated muscles
Aventant et al (2010)
Watching painful stimuli applied to ingroup (same-race) models led to a significant reduction in MEPs from the FDI muscle
muscle corresponding to the observed pain site.
No MEP reduction was found when watching outgroup (other-race) models in pain.
No differences were found in the ADM muscle (a control muscle not involved in the pain), confirming muscle specificity.
P’s race (Black or White) did not influence the results — both groups showed similar patterns.
suggests that sensorimotor empathy is both muscle-specific and socially biased, showing stronger neural mirroring for ingroup pain.
Aventant et al (2010) violet skin effect
Painful stimulation to ingroup hands led to a significant reduction in MEP amplitude in the FDI muscle (the observed pain site).
No significant MEP reduction was found for the ADM muscle (not stimulated), regardless of group — confirming muscle-specificity.
A mixed-model ANOVA showed no interaction or main effect of participant race — both Black and White participants showed similar MEP modulation.
MEP reduction was greater for:
Ingroup models > Outgroup
Violet model > Outgroup
Mino-Palvello et al (2008) AS & empathy
Individuals w/AS showed no differences in sensorimotor empathy across the stimuli presented
neurotypical had a large decrease in MEP for pain only → showing empathy for pain
AS didn't significantly show reduced MEP for pain
didnt show the index of empathy
Adam & Galinksy (2012) exp1 results
P who wore a lab coat made fewer errors on incongruent trials than those who did not wear a lab coat
those wearing a lab coat made about half as many errors as those not
No difference in errors was found between the two groups on non-incongruent trials.
Adam & Galinksy (2012) exp2 results
P who wore a doctor’s coat found more differences than those who wore a painter’s coat or only saw a doctor's coat
no significant difference in performance between P who saw a doctor’s coat & those who wore a painter’s coat.
Wearing a doctor’s coat may have enhanced sustained attention, possibly because of the symbolic association with careful, detail-oriented professions (like medicine).
Simply seeing a doctor’s coat was not enough to boost attention → wearing it was necessary to see the benefits.
Wearing a painter’s coat did not produce the same effect, suggesting that the meaning of the clothing matters in cognitive performance.
Adam & Galinksy (2012) exp3 results
P who wore a doctor’s coat, found significantly more differences than those who identified with a doctor's coat → simply identifying with the idea of a doctor's coat wasn’t as effective as actually wearing it
P in the identifying-with-a-doctor’s-coat condition found more differences than those wearing a painter’s coat → suggests that priming did increase attention somewhat, but not as much as actually wearing it.
P in the painter’s coat condition performed the worst, reinforcing the idea that the symbolic meaning of clothing matters.
example of divided attention (Strayer & Johnston, 2001)
a simulated driving task → allowed them to examine the effects of engaging in cell phone conversation on various aspects of driving performance
results showed P who were involved in cell phone conversations were more prone to missing stimulated traffic signals & exhibited slower reaction times
degree of impairment was consistent regardless of whether phone was used in a handheld or hands free mode
cell phone convo disrupts driving performance by diverting attention to a cognitively engaging task that competes w/mental focus required of safe driving
illustrates dangers of multitasking within complex environments
Civile & Obhi (2017) exp 1
p wearing uniforms had slower RT overall (poorer task performance)
suggests that wearing uniform affects cognitive processing
no significant difference between black/white faces
distractor not race
implies uniform was responsible for reduced performance
wearing uniform primes wearer to be more vigilant leading to greater distraction
Civile & Obhi (2017) exp 2
faster RT in congruent trials suggest attention was already directed there
attentional bias more attentions drawn
p wearing police uniform showed attentional bias for hoodies (quicker RT if dot behind)
no significant attentional bias between back/white faces
uniform increased attentional capture by clothes associated w/perceived threat
Civile & Obhi (2017) exp 3
attentional bias towards hoodies (Low SES) only occurred when p wore police uniform
attentional bias based on perceived social class
simply seeing uniform had no effect
no difference between black vs white
police uniforms & bias towards to lower SES are linked
supports enclothed cognition
Heeren et al (2017) results
show attentional bias for social threat
shown in probe detection/discrimination task
faster response to problem replacing threat stimuli
not present in non-anxious
suggest cognitive mechanisms maintains anxiety
Mclaren et al (2021)
Young children show latent inhibition from simple pre-exposure, consistent with slower learning.
Older groups do not, suggesting developmental changes in attentional control or learning systems.
Supports the idea that experience with stimuli can interfere with learning, especially in early development.
Rodriguez exp 1 (2010)
Female rats spent more time in the landmark area than males.
This indicates that females relied more on the visual landmark, while
Males tended to rely more on the geometric shape of the pool.
There is a sex difference in spatial cue preference:
Females → prefer landmarks
Males → prefer geometric configuration
Rodriguez exp 2 (2010)
Females showed stronger preference for the landmark cue.
Both sexes successfully used both types of cues when tested individually
difference is preferential, not an inability – not an innate limitation.
Civile & Chamizo et al (2014) exp 1
Configural arrangement of landmarks is crucial for spatial memory.
Males navigated better overall
flipping near landmarks disrupted performance more than flipping far ones
Civile & Chamizo et al (2014) exp 2
performance was above chance in control & flipped far
rats in inverted did not perform well → disrupted navigation
spent less time in z due to confusion
Civile, Chamizo, (2020) exp1a
best performance in normal conditions
worst performance in inverted
rats struggled when spatial arrangement is flipped
Z alone isnt sufficient
rely on Z & near (B & C)
Civile, Chamizo, (2020) exp1b
B & C alone had weak effect
contributed navigation but less affective without z
Z alone provided strong guidance
rats stayed in quadrant