Case Studies

0.0(0)
studied byStudied by 0 people
learnLearn
examPractice Test
spaced repetitionSpaced Repetition
heart puzzleMatch
flashcardsFlashcards
Card Sorting

1/10

Study Analytics
Name
Mastery
Learn
Test
Matching
Spaced

No study sessions yet.

11 Terms

1
New cards

Study: Steele and Aronson (1995)

Aim: To investigate the effect of stereotype threat on African American students’ performance on standardized tests.

Procedure and Results:
Steele and Aronson asked African American and White college students to take a difficult verbal test under two conditions. In one condition, the test was described as diagnostic of intellectual ability (activating stereotype threat), and in the other, it was presented as a non-diagnostic task. Results showed that African American students performed significantly worse under the diagnostic condition compared to White students and compared to their own performance in the non-diagnostic condition. This suggested that anxiety about confirming negative stereotypes impaired their test performance.

Analysis

Strengths:

  1. Internal Validity: The controlled experimental conditions and precise operationalization of stereotype threat ensured that the study effectively isolated the variable under investigation.

  2. Reliability: The study's design has been replicated in multiple follow-up studies with consistent results, underscoring its reliability.

  3. Credibility: The use of objective measures, such as standardized test scores, adds to the credibility and accuracy of the findings.

  4. Generalizability within Context: While limited to academic testing, the findings provide valuable insights into how stereotype threat operates in structured, high-stakes environments.

Weaknesses:

  1. Ecological Validity: The laboratory setting does not fully capture the complexity of stereotype threats in real-world settings, where multiple factors may interact.

  2. Generalizability: The study focused primarily on American racial groups and academic contexts, limiting its broader applicability to other cultural or situational settings.

  3. Bias Potential: Participant awareness of the experimental setup could introduce demand characteristics, potentially influencing results.

  4. External Validity: Critics have noted that the effect size of stereotype threat varies across different contexts and populations, which could limit the study's external validity in predicting real-world outcomes.

2
New cards

Study: Tajfel et al. (1970)

Aim: To investigate if mere categorization into groups would lead to ingroup bias and discrimination.

Procedure and Results:
Tajfel randomly assigned boys to groups based on trivial criteria (e.g., preference for paintings) with no interaction between groups. The boys then allocated points (rewards) to members of their own group and the outgroup. Despite no real conflict or competition, boys consistently favored their own group, giving them more points even when it meant smaller absolute rewards, showing ingroup bias and discrimination.

Analysis

Strengths:

  1. Internal Validity: The minimal group paradigm successfully isolated social categorization as a variable, ensuring the study directly tested ingroup bias without confounding factors.

  2. Reliability: The controlled setup and simplicity of the design allow for easy replication, with consistent findings across similar studies supporting reliability.

  3. Credibility: The clear operationalization of group favoritism through point allocation provides an objective and quantifiable measure of ingroup bias.

  4. Construct Validity: The study effectively demonstrates how even trivial group distinctions can lead to significant psychological effects, enhancing its theoretical relevance.

Weaknesses:

  1. Ecological Validity: The artificial laboratory conditions and minimal group context may not fully capture the complexity of real-world social group dynamics.

  2. Generalizability: The sample of British schoolboys limits applicability across different ages, cultures, and genders, reducing external validity.

  3. Bias Potential: Participant awareness of the experimental nature might have influenced their behavior, potentially introducing demand characteristics.

  4. Context-Specific Application: While the study illuminates basic ingroup bias mechanisms, it may not fully explain discrimination in situations involving deeply rooted social identities or historical conflicts.

3
New cards

Study: Berry (1967)

Aim: To investigate conformity levels across different cultures with varying social norms.

Procedure and Results:
Participants were asked to complete standard conformity tasks where they had to match line lengths while being exposed to group pressure. The study compared the rates of conformity between the two groups to explore how cultural norms influenced conformity behaviour. He found that the Temne showed higher conformity rates compared to the Inuit, suggesting that cultural norms emphasizing community and cooperation increase conformity behaviour.

Analysis

Strengths:

  1. Ecological Validity: By studying real cultural groups, the research provides a meaningful insight into how cultural norms influence behavior in natural contexts.

  2. Cross-Cultural Comparison: The study highlights the role of collectivist and individualist norms in shaping conformity, contributing to the understanding of cultural dimensions.

  3. Reliability: The replication of Asch’s conformity experiment with standardized tasks allows for consistency and comparability of results.

  4. Construct Validity: The operationalization of conformity through measurable responses ensures the concept is directly addressed.

Weaknesses:

  1. Control Issues: Cross-cultural research faces challenges in controlling extraneous variables, such as participants’ education levels or exposure to Western norms, which may influence results.

  2. Generalizability: Small and non-representative sample sizes from each culture limit the ability to apply findings broadly across other collectivist or individualist societies.

  3. Ethnocentric Bias: Using Western-designed experimental tasks may fail to capture culturally specific understandings of conformity, affecting construct validity.

  4. Credibility: Differences in the context and interpretation of the task across cultures could lead to misrepresentation of behavior or cultural norms.

4
New cards

Study: Hilliard and Liben (2010)

Aim: To investigate how children’s gender stereotypes about occupations develop through enculturation.

Procedure:
Hilliard and Liben asked children aged 5-6 and 9-10 to identify which jobs they thought were appropriate for boys or girls. The study used interviews and questionnaires to explore how children categorize occupations by gender. This allowed the researchers to compare differences in gender stereotyping across age groups.

Results:
Younger children held more rigid and traditional gender stereotypes about occupations, strongly associating certain jobs with boys or girls. Older children showed more flexibility, indicating an increasing understanding that gender roles are not fixed. These findings suggest that enculturation through social and cultural influences gradually shapes children’s gender-related beliefs.

Analysis

Strengths:

  1. Generalizability: The study included a diverse sample of children, enhancing the applicability of findings across various subgroups within the U.S. population.

  2. Developmental Insight: Comparing younger and older children provided valuable information on how gender stereotypes evolve over time, reflecting the impact of enculturation.

  3. Reliability: The use of direct questioning and structured questionnaires ensured consistent data collection methods, increasing the study's reliability.

  4. Internal Validity: Controlled settings and clearly defined tasks reduced extraneous variables, making the study’s findings more robust.

Weaknesses:

  1. Social Desirability Bias: Children might have provided responses they thought adults wanted to hear, potentially reducing the credibility of the data.

  2. Limited Generalizability: The study’s cultural context (American children) restricts its application to other cultures where gender roles may differ.

  3. Cross-Sectional Design: The study could not track individual developmental changes over time, limiting its ability to establish causality in the evolution of gender stereotypes.

  4. Ecological Validity: The artificial research setting and structured nature of the tasks may not fully capture how children apply gender stereotypes in real-world contexts.

5
New cards

Study: Miranda and Matheny (2000)

Aim: To explore factors contributing to acculturative stress in Hispanic immigrants.

Procedure and Results:
Using an online questionnaire, the study assessed cultural adaptation, language proficiency, and social support among Hispanic immigrants. Results indicated that high social support reduced acculturative stress, while language barriers increased it. This study showed individual differences in acculturation outcomes.

Analysis

Strengths:

  1. Generalizability: The large and diverse sample of Hispanic immigrants improves the applicability of the findings across different subgroups within the population.

  2. Reliability: Quantitative data from structured questionnaires allowed measurable and consistent evaluation of relationships, enhancing the study’s reliability.

  3. Convenience and Accessibility: The online format facilitated access to participants from varied geographical areas, broadening the study’s reach and representativeness.

  4. Internal Validity: The study focused on well-defined factors like social support and language proficiency, providing clear operationalization for analysis.

Weaknesses:

  1. Social Desirability Bias: Self-reported data might have been influenced by participants’ desire to present themselves positively, reducing credibility.

  2. Limited Depth: The use of quantitative methods alone restricts the exploration of personal narratives and the nuanced experiences of acculturation, limiting ecological validity.

  3. Cross-Sectional Design: The snapshot nature of the study prevents analysis of changes in acculturation and stress over time, impacting longitudinal insights.

  4. Cultural Bias: The focus on Hispanic immigrants may not account for acculturative stress factors that differ across other cultural groups, limiting generalizability to non-Hispanic populations.

6
New cards

Study 1: Joy, Kimball, and Zabrack (1986)

 Aim: To investigate the effect of viewing televised aggression on children's aggressive behaviour.
Procedure:
Children were shown violent television programs and then observed during free play to measure their aggressive behaviours. The researchers recorded instances of physical and verbal aggression exhibited by the children after exposure to the violent content. This design aimed to directly link media exposure to changes in behaviour.

Results:
Children who watched the violent programs displayed significantly more aggressive acts than those who watched non-violent content. The findings support the idea that children learn and imitate aggressive behaviours through observation, as predicted by social cognitive theory. This demonstrated that media violence can increase aggression in young viewers.

Strengths:

  1. Internal Validity: The experimental design with control groups and random assignment strengthened the cause-and-effect relationship between televised aggression and children’s aggressive behaviour.

  2. Operationalization: Aggression was clearly defined and directly observed, making the data objective and reliable.

  3. Causal Evidence: This study provided strong support for social cognitive theory by demonstrating that exposure to violent media can lead to increased aggression, supporting theoretical validity.

  4. Replicability: The clear procedure allows for replication, enhancing reliability across studies.

Weaknesses:

  1. Ecological Validity: The artificial lab environment may not accurately reflect real-world media consumption or natural play, limiting generalizability.

  2. Temporal Limitation: Observations focused on immediate effects, so the study does not reveal whether aggression persists or changes over time, affecting longitudinal validity.

  3. Ethical Concerns: Exposing children to violent media raises ethical issues about potential harm and long-term psychological effects, challenging the study’s moral acceptability.

  4. Demand Characteristics: Children might have altered behaviour due to awareness of being observed, potentially biasing results and reducing credibility.

7
New cards

Study 2: Charlton et al. (2002)

 Aim: To examine behavioural changes after introduction of television to a previously TV-free community (St. Helena).
Procedure:
Researchers conducted naturalistic observations of children’s behaviour in the island community of St. Helena before and after television was introduced. They recorded levels of aggression and prosocial behaviour over an extended period. This allowed them to assess the impact of TV exposure in a real-world setting with minimal experimental manipulation.

Results:
Contrary to expectations, no significant increase in aggressive behaviour was observed after the introduction of television. The findings suggested that strong community social norms and adult supervision acted as protective factors against imitating aggressive behaviour seen on TV. This indicated that environmental and cultural factors influence whether social cognitive theory’s predictions hold true.

Strengths:

  1. Ecological Validity: The naturalistic, real-world setting of St. Helena increased the ecological validity, making findings more applicable to everyday life.

  2. Longitudinal Design: Observing behaviour before and after TV introduction over time allowed researchers to track actual behavioural changes, improving temporal validity.

  3. Contextual Insight: The study highlighted the protective role of social norms and adult supervision, showing the importance of cultural context in moderating media effects, enhancing theoretical depth.

  4. Natural Experiment: The lack of direct manipulation reduced artificial influences on behaviour, increasing the credibility of observed results.

Weaknesses:

  1. Low Experimental Control: Without controlled conditions, it is difficult to establish causality between TV introduction and behaviour changes, limiting internal validity.

  2. Limited Generalizability: The small, isolated community sample may not represent larger, more diverse populations, restricting external validity.

  3. Confounding Variables: Other social changes occurring at the same time as TV introduction might have influenced behaviour, reducing the study’s reliability and interpretive clarity.

  4. Observer Bias: Researchers’ interpretations during observation may introduce subjective bias, affecting the credibility of findings.

8
New cards

Study 2: Chen et al. (2005)

 Aim: To examine cultural differences in negotiation style linked to individualism and collectivism.
Procedure:
Chen et al. conducted negotiation simulations involving American and Chinese participants to observe how cultural backgrounds influence negotiation style. Participants engaged in role-play scenarios designed to mimic real-life business negotiations. The study compared communication approaches between individualistic American and collectivistic Chinese cultures.

Results:
The results showed that American participants tended to be more direct, assertive, and task-focused during negotiations. In contrast, Chinese participants emphasized maintaining relationships, used more indirect communication, and prioritized group harmony. These findings illustrated how cultural dimensions like individualism and collectivism shape cognitive and behavioural patterns in social interactions.

Strengths:

  1. Ecological Validity: Using negotiation simulations modeled on real-life scenarios increases the study’s relevance to actual social and business interactions.

  2. Comparative Cross-Cultural Design: The clear comparison between American and Chinese participants helps isolate cultural influences on negotiation styles, enhancing internal validity.

  3. Mixed Methods: Combining qualitative observations with quantitative data provides a richer, more nuanced understanding of cultural behaviour, improving credibility and depth.

Weaknesses:

  1. Ecological Limitations: Simulated negotiations may lack the complexity and stakes of real-world negotiations, limiting ecological validity and generalizability.

  2. Sampling Bias: The use of student participants restricts generalizability, as they may not represent the wider populations of their cultures, affecting external validity.

  3. Potential Cultural Stereotyping: Participants might respond based on cultural expectations or stereotypes, introducing social desirability bias and reducing credibility.

9
New cards

Study 2: Fagot (1978)

 Aim: To study parental reinforcement of gender roles in early childhood.
Procedure:
Fagot observed parents interacting with their toddlers, focusing on how they responded to the children’s gender-appropriate and gender-inappropriate behaviours. The study involved naturalistic observation in the home setting to capture genuine parental reactions. Researchers recorded instances of praise or correction based on the child’s actions relative to traditional gender norms.

Results:
The results indicated that parents consistently praised behaviours aligned with traditional gender roles and corrected or discouraged behaviours that were gender-inconsistent. This parental reinforcement helped shape the child’s understanding of gender-appropriate behaviour and social expectations. The study highlights the role of parental enculturation in influencing early gender cognition and behaviour development.

Strengths:

  1. Ecological Validity: The use of naturalistic observation in home settings captures authentic parental behaviour, making findings more representative of real-life interactions.

  2. Clear Behavioural Evidence: Observing direct parental responses to gendered behaviours provides credible support for the role of enculturation in gender development.

  3. Rich Qualitative Data: Detailed recordings of praise and correction offer nuanced insights into how gender norms are transmitted in early childhood.

Weaknesses:

  1. Causal Inference Limited: As an observational study without experimental manipulation, it cannot definitively establish causality between parental responses and child behaviour, affecting internal validity.

  2. Cultural Bias and Generalizability: The study’s focus on a single cultural context limits its applicability to other cultures, reducing external validity and generalizability.

  3. Observer Bias and Reliability: Researcher interpretation during observations may introduce bias, and without standardized coding, reliability could be compromised.

  4. Potential Demand Characteristics: Parents might alter behaviour knowing they are being observed, threatening credibility through social desirability bias.

10
New cards

Study 1: Becker et al. (2002)

Aim: To study the influence of Western media on eating behaviours in Fijian adolescent girls.
Procedure:
Becker and her team conducted a longitudinal study in Fiji, focusing on adolescent girls' eating behaviours and attitudes toward body image before and after the introduction of Western television. Researchers used surveys and semi-structured interviews to assess changes in disordered eating patterns and exposure to Western media. Observations were conducted over several years to track how cultural values shifted in response to globalization.

Results:
The findings revealed a significant increase in eating disorder symptoms, such as body dissatisfaction and dieting behaviours, following the introduction of television. Participants often cited the influence of Western beauty standards portrayed on TV as a source of pressure to conform to thinner ideals. The study demonstrated the impact of globalization on reshaping traditional cultural attitudes towards body image and eating practices.

Strengths:

  1. Longitudinal Design: Tracking participants over several years enhances the study’s ability to observe behavioural and attitudinal changes over time, strengthening internal validity related to temporal effects.

  2. Ecological Validity: Conducting research in a naturalistic, real-world setting (Fiji) increases ecological validity, as behaviours and attitudes are observed in participants’ actual cultural environment.

  3. Rich Mixed Methods: Using both surveys and semi-structured interviews improves credibility by combining quantitative and qualitative data, capturing both measurable trends and personal experiences.

  4. Relevance and Real-World Impact: The study highlights significant social and cultural implications of globalization, making it valuable for understanding media influence cross-culturally.

Weaknesses:

  1. Lack of Control Group: Without a comparison group unaffected by television introduction, the study cannot definitively establish causality between media exposure and increased eating disorder symptoms, reducing internal validity.

  2. Confounding Variables: Other concurrent social changes (e.g., economic or educational shifts) could also affect eating behaviours, potentially biasing results and challenging the reliability of attributing change solely to media exposure.

  3. Generalizability Limitations: The focus on adolescent girls in Fiji limits external validity, as findings may not generalize to other cultures, age groups, or genders.

  4. Potential Reporting Bias: Self-reported surveys and interviews are vulnerable to social desirability bias, which may affect credibility and accuracy of the reported eating behaviours.

11
New cards

Study 2: Ogihara and Uchida (2014)

 Aim: To investigate cultural value shifts in Japanese society due to globalization.
Procedure:
Ogihara and Uchida conducted surveys to measure individualism and collectivism among Japanese participants, focusing on generational differences. They used standardized scales to evaluate shifts in values, comparing responses from younger and older age groups over time. The study also examined correlations between individualism and subjective well-being, exploring how globalization influenced attitudes and behaviours.

Results:
Findings indicated a marked increase in individualistic values among younger Japanese participants, reflecting a shift away from traditional collectivist norms. However, this rise in individualism was associated with decreased well-being due to conflicts with deeply rooted societal expectations of collectivism. The results highlighted globalization’s role in reshaping cultural values and its psychological consequences in a traditionally collectivist society.

Strengths:

  1. Large, Representative Sample: Enhances the generalizability of findings within Japanese society, improving external validity.

  2. Longitudinal Data Use: Tracking generational changes over time allows for a better understanding of cultural value shifts and strengthens the study’s internal validity regarding temporal trends.

  3. Standardized Measures: Use of validated scales for individualism and collectivism supports reliability and comparability with other studies.

  4. Focus on Psychological Outcomes: Linking cultural shifts to subjective well-being provides valuable insight into the real-world consequences of globalization on mental health.

Weaknesses:

  1. Self-Report Surveys: Dependence on self-reported data increases the risk of social desirability bias and inaccuracies, reducing the credibility and validity of responses.

  2. Limited Cultural Complexity: Surveys may oversimplify complex cultural constructs like individualism and collectivism, potentially missing nuanced attitudes and behaviours, thus limiting construct validity.

  3. Cross-Cultural Influences: The study focuses only on Japanese society, so the findings may not generalize to other cultures experiencing globalization differently.

  4. Correlation vs. Causation: While trends and associations are shown, the design does not establish causality between globalization and value shifts or well-being outcomes.