Results for "mary rothbart"

Flashcards

flashcards Flashcard (56)
studied byStudied by 4 people
268 days ago
0.0(0)
flashcards Flashcard (56)
studied byStudied by 21 people
748 days ago
0.0(0)

Notes

note Note
studied byStudied by 1 person
28 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 5 people
29 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 0 people
41 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 0 people
42 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 0 people
43 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 0 people
64 days ago
0.0(0)
Neuroscience Research on Attention During recent decades, researchers have developed a variety of sophisticated techniques for examining the biological basis of behavior; Chapter 1 introduced many of these approaches. Research using these techniques has identified a network of areas through- out the brain that accomplish various attention tasks (Posner & Rothbart, 2007b). Several regions of the brain are responsible for attention. In this discussion, however, we’ll focus on structures in the cerebral cortex, as shown in Figure 3.2. Take a moment to compare Figure 3.2 with Figure 2.1 (p. 38), which shows the regions of the cortex that are most relevant in object recognition. According to Michael Posner and Mary Rothbart, several systems in the cortex process different aspects of attention (Posner & Rothbart, 2007a, 2007b; Rothbart et al., 2011; Tang & Posner, 2009). We will discuss two of them, the orienting attention network and the executive attention network. The Orienting Attention Network. Imagine that you are searching the area around your bathroom sink for a lost contact lens. When you are selecting information from sensory input, your orienting attention network is activated. The orienting 84 CHAPTER 3 Perceptual Processes II: Attention and Consciousness FIGURE 3.2 A Schematic Drawing of the Cerebral Cortex, as Seen from the Left Side, Showing the Four Lobes of the Brain and the Regions that Are Most Important on Attention Tasks. Parietal lobe Frontal lobe Orienting Attention Network Executive Attention Network Occipital lobe Temporal lobe attention network is generally responsible for the kind of attention required for visual search, in which you must shift your attention around to various spatial locations (Chun & Wolfe, 2001; Posner & Rothbart, 2007b). Figure 3.2 shows that two important components of the orienting attention network are located in the region of the cortex known as the parietal lobe (pronounced ‘‘pah-rie-uh-tul’’). How did researchers identify the parietal cortex as the region of the brain that we use in visual searches? Several decades ago, the only clue to the organization of the brain was provided by people with brain lesions (Posner, 2004). The term brain lesion refers to specific brain damage caused by strokes, accidents, or other traumas. People who have brain damage in the parietal region of the right hemisphere of the brain have trouble noticing a visual stimulus that appears on the left side of their visual field. In contrast, people with damage in the left parietal region have trouble noticing a visual stimulus on the right side (Luck & Vecera, 2002; Posner & Rothbart, 2007a, 2007b; Styles, 2005). Neurologists use the term unilateral spatial neglect when a person ignores part of his or her visual field (Gazzaniga et al., 2009). Explanations for Attention 85 These lesions produce unusual deficits. For instance, a woman with a lesion in the left parietal region may have trouble noticing the food on the right side of her plate. She may eat only the food on the left side of her plate, and she might even complain that she didn’t receive enough food (Farah, 2000; Humphreys & Riddoch, 2001). Surprisingly, however, she may seem completely unaware of her deficit. In another study, researchers tested a man with a lesion in his right parietal lobe. They showed him a figure of an ordinary clock and asked him to draw it. His drawing showed only half of the clock, with the other half completely empty (Bloom & Lazerson, 1988). Some of the more recent research on the orienting attention network has used PET scans. In studies using positron emission tomography (abbreviated PET scan), the researchers measure blood flow in the brain by injecting the participant with a radioactive chemical just before he or she performs a cognitive task. As discussed in Chapter 1, this chemical travels through the blood to the parts of the brain that are active during the cognitive task. A specialized camera makes an image of the accumulated chemical. According to PET-scan research, the parietal cortex shows increased blood flow when people perform visual searches and when they pay attention to spatial locations (Posner & Rothbart, 2007b). The orienting network develops during the first year of life. For example, by about 4 months of age, infants can disengage their attention from an overstimulating situation, and they can shift this attention to an object such as a new toy (Posner & Rothbart, 2007a; Rothbart et al., 2011). The Executive Attention Network. The Stroop task that you tried in Demon- stration 3.1 relied primarily on your executive attention network. The executive attention network is responsible for the kind of attention we use when a task focuses on conflict (Posner & Rothbart, 2007a, 2007b). On the Stroop task, for example, you need to inhibit your automatic response of reading a word, so that you can name the color of the ink (Fan et al., 2002). More generally, the executive attention network inhibits your automatic responses to stimuli (Stuss et al., 2002). As you can see in Figure 3.2, the prefrontal portion of the cortex is the region of your brain where the executive attention network is especially active. The executive attention network is primarily involved during top-down control of attention (Farah, 2000). This network begins to develop at about age 3, much later than the orienting attention network (Posner & Rothbart, 2007a; Rothbart et al., 2011). Posner and Rothbart (2007b) argue that the executive attention network is extremely important when you acquire academic skills in school, for example, when you learned to read. There is also some evidence that adults can enhance their executive attention network by learning meditation, adopted from traditional Chinese techniques (Tang & Posner, 2009). Executive attention also helps you learn new ideas (Posner & Rothbart, 2007a). For example, as you are reading this passage, your executive attention network has been actively taking in new information. Ideally, you have also been comparing this executive attention network with the orienting attention network. This process of reading and understanding a college-level textbook can be challenging. Not 86 CHAPTER 3 Perceptual Processes II: Attention and Consciousness surprisingly, the location of the executive attention network overlaps with the areas of your brain that are related to general intelligence (Duncan et al., 2000; Posner & Rothbart, 2007b). In summary, PET scans and other neuroscience techniques have identified one brain region that is typically active when we are searching for objects (the orienting attention network). Neuroscience research also show that a different brain region is typically active when we must inhibit an automatic response and produce a less obvious response (the executive attention network); this second network is also active in academic learning.
note Note
studied byStudied by 2 people
78 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 0 people
92 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 1 person
92 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 0 people
113 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 0 people
134 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 0 people
327 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 1 person
679 days ago
0.0(0)
note Note
studied byStudied by 214 people
705 days ago
4.9(7)
note Note
studied byStudied by 52 people
705 days ago
4.5(2)