Newborns prefer to listen to speech over non-speech
New cards
2
Vouloumanos et al (2010)
Preference for human speech over primate vocalisation appears after 3 months
New cards
3
Ramus et al. (2000)
Newborns can discriminate languages but not when the recording is backwards
New cards
4
Jusczyk, Cutler & Redanz (1993)
American 9-month-olds prefer to listen to low-pass filtered strong/weak words over weak/strong words
New cards
5
Jusczyk et al. (1993)
American and dutch 9-month-olds listen longer to their native words that feature language-specific segmental phonotactics - but not if low-pass filtered.
New cards
6
Jusczyk, Luce & Charles-Luce (1994)
English-learning 9-month-olds listen longer to nonsense syllables with high phonotactic probabilities
New cards
7
Swingley & Aslin (2002)
14-month-olds look less at the corresponding picture if the word is 'mispronounced' by a single feature
New cards
8
Maye, Werker, & Gerken (2002)
6-month-olds can perform distributional learning from phonetic input
New cards
9
Aitchison & Chiat (1981)
4- to 9-year-olds produce 'consonant harmony' in recall of newly learned words. However, more likely to be a failure of memory rather than articulation.
New cards
10
Brent & Siskind (2001)
Only a small portion of utterances in infant-directed speech consist of a single word
New cards
11
Saffran, Aslin, & Newport (1996)
Infants looked longer at the side that was playing part words. Novelty effect.
New cards
12
Cutler & Carter (1987)
The most common prosodic word type in English is strong-weak disyllables.
New cards
13
Kelly & Martin (1994)
Strong syllable = word onset 95% of the time in IDS
New cards
14
Jusczyk, Houston & Newsome (1999)
7.5-month-old infants use predominant prosodic patterns to segment words
New cards
15
Bortfeld et al. (2005)
Infants can use familiar words for further segmentation
New cards
16
Casello et al. (1999)
Noun bias in early lexical development, possibly due to the whole object assumption
New cards
17
Landau, Smith & Jones (1988)
Shape bias makes children extend the referent of a noun based on its shape
New cards
18
Smith and Yu (2008)
Children can use co-occurrence probabilities to perform cross-situational learning
New cards
19
Tomasello & Barton (1994)
2-year-olds can determine the referent of an unknown word based on their understanding of speaker intention (theory of mind)
New cards
20
Akhtar et al. (1996)
2-year-olds can also determine the referent of an unknown word based on their understanding of the speaker's knowledge state.
New cards
21
Markman (1990)
Mechanisms behind word-learning are specific to language. Evidence: shape bias only observed when words are involved.
New cards
22
Bloom (2000)
Mechanisms behind word-learning apply generally. Evidence: children can fast-map word-meaning as well as facts. Mutual exclusivity works both for word-learning and fact-learning.
New cards
23
Dual Route Model
All inflected forms learnt through memory. The child over applies a default rule to irregulars. Then irregular forms are learned and block rule application.
New cards
24
Single Route Model
Stores some past-tense forms in memory. Forms with infrequent patterns adopt similar patterns due to analogy. The child recovers from over-regularisation through reinforced memory of irregular patterns.
New cards
25
Rumelhart & McLelland (1986)
Fed a computer verbs (10 high frequency for 10 trials, 410 medium for 190). Then tested network on present tenses and saw what past-tense form it supplied.
New cards
26
Marcus et al. (1992)
The emergence of -ed marking is sudden, and coincides with over-regularisation. This shows that -ed is acquired as a rule (which overapplies)
New cards
27
Marchman (1997)
Regular verbs are not entirely protected from frequency effects. Children sometimes over-irregularise when there are many similar-sounding irregulars.
New cards
28
Clahsen & Almazan (1998)
Children with WS show deficiency in irregular past-tense verbs, but not regular ones. Rule and memory are different components.
New cards
29
Gopnik & Crago (1991)
Children with SLI show deficiency in regular past-tense verbs but not in irregular ones. Rule and memory are different components.
New cards
30
McClelland & Patterson (2002)
A phonological deficit can explain some of the dissociation (consonant clusters). SR Model
New cards
31
Gerken & McIntosh (1993)
Children can find the correct picture better when they are told "Find the dog for me" than "Find was dog for me"
New cards
32
Dye et al. (2018)
10-month-olds prefer "the dog" over "dog the"
New cards
33
Constructivist accounts
Syntactic knowledge develops gradually from concrete item-based learning to abstract structural knowledge.
New cards
34
Generative approach
Abstract categories are innate (includes lexical and functional). Children are aware early on of the relationship between functional and lexical categories.
New cards
35
Pine and Lieven (1997)
No overlap in the use of DET with the same nouns (item-based)
New cards
36
Valian et al (2009)
Children's overlap in DET use is about 50% (same as adults)
New cards
37
Yang (2010)
Cannot expect high levels of overlap in spontaneous speech because we don't always recombine articles for every noun.
New cards
38
Semantic bootstrapping
Meaning-category correspondence guides the initial phase of category and relationship learning in syntax.
New cards
39
Pinker (1984)
Children are innately equipped with knowledge of the mapping between syntactic category types and functions on one hand and semantic types on the other.
New cards
40
Phonological/prosodic bootstrapping
Phonological and prosodic cues aid the learning of syntactic categories and phrases. Syntactic boundaries are marked by prosodic boundaries.
New cards
41
Shi et al. (1998)
Function words tend to have shorter vowel durations, weaker amplitude, and simpler syllable structure than lexical words.
New cards
42
Kelly (1988)
In English, disyllabic nouns tend to have initial stress while disyllabic verbs tend to have final stress.
New cards
43
Distributional learning
A word's syntactic category and its grammatical role in a sentence can be learned by looking at the words that surround it, its morphological marking, and its relative position in a sentence. It goes beyond the frequency of occurrence of a particular word.
New cards
44
Wexler (1998)
Very Early Parameter Setting. Head directionality is set very early.
New cards
45
Akhtar & Tomasello (1997)
2- to 3-year-olds' verb use closely parallels what they have heard. Even if that means accepting word order configurations inconsistent with the target language
New cards
46
Gertner et al. (2006)
Even 21-month-olds performed above chance with novel verbs in preferential looking procedure
New cards
47
Fernandes et al. (2006)
28- and 34-month-olds can generalise from neutral and intransitive sentences.
New cards
48
Savage et al. (2003)
Children's structural priming seems to be lexically based (at least before age 4)
New cards
49
Radford (1996)
Children's early grammars are truncated versions of the adult grammar (small clauses)
New cards
50
Rizzi (1993/4)
Children project all the necessary functional categories available in the adult grammar but children do not necessarily know the obligatoriness of these categories (Truncation Account)
New cards
51
ATOM: Wexler (1998)
Agreement/Tense Omission Model. Children's early grammars project all functional categories, parameters are set from the earliest stages. Children optionally produce them due to a maturational constraint.
New cards
52
Unique Checking Constraint (UCC)
Check the features of a verb against only one functional category -> an item can be checked at AGR or TNS but not both.
New cards
53
MOSAIC: Freudenthal et al. (2007)
Model of syntax acquisition in children. Empiricist/Usage-based approaches: Children learn strings of language directly from the input.
New cards
54
Freudenthal et al. (2007)
Recency effect: Good correlation between utterance-final non-finites in the input and RIs in children.
New cards
55
Legate & Yang (2007)
Variational model. Combines presence of categories (=/-TNS) with nature of the input. Utterances with (overtly marked) tense/agreement reward + TNS. Captures crosslinguistic data by combining categories with input data.
New cards
56
Nativism (G)
Most core aspects of syntactic knowledge are part of Universal Grammar and innate
New cards
57
Development (G)
Involves selecting the options set by UG and learning language-specific properties of syntax outside UG
New cards
58
Continuity (G)
What underlies children's sentence is the same kind of abstract grammar that adults have.
New cards
59
Non-adultlike production/comprehension (G)
Either they haven't set UG to the correct settings, they are still learning idiosyncratic properties of the language, or committing performance errors.
New cards
60
Emergentism (C)
Much of syntactic knowledge is learned from the input
New cards
61
Development (C)
Involves learning concrete exemplars of word combinations, and then gradually making generalisations over such exemplars.
New cards
62
Discontinuity (C)
What underlies young children's sentence is item-based and not the kind of abstract grammar that adults have.
New cards
63
Non-adultlike production.comprehension (C)
Because children start by learning fixed combinations, they sometimes overapply them to inappropriate contexts.
New cards
64
Goodluck & Tavakolian (1982)
Children have adultlike syntactic knowledge but are not good at processing sentences in experimental tasks. (G)
New cards
65
Diessel & Tomasello (2000)
Children's early relative clauses are just amalgams of a prefabricated schema and a bare untensed VP. (C)
New cards
66
Kidd et al. (2007)
Children are better at processing types of relative clauses that they hear or say the most (C)
New cards
67
Santelman et al. (2002)
Do-support is an idiosyncratic feature of English (outside UG) that just needs to be learned (G)
New cards
68
Guasti et al. (1995)
English-learning children mis-set the negation parameter to the 'do not raise' setting (G)
New cards
69
Rowland & Pine (2000)
Children are accurate in production of items that are frequent in the input, and inaccurate for those that aren't
New cards
70
Ambridge & Rowland (2009)
Children who have have the 'What does [THING] [PROCESS]" schema produce doubling errors, children who say "What she does like?", don't. (C)
New cards
71
Trueswell & Gleitman (2007)
Children become sensitive to phrasal ordering predictors (word order), lexcially specific predictors, and less reliable semantic, contextual and referential predictors.
New cards
72
Trueswell et al. (1999)
Put the frog on the napkin in the box. The Kindergarten Path Effect.
New cards
73
Late closure
When possible, attach incoming lexical items into the clause or phrase currently being processed
New cards
74
Minimal attachment
Attach incoming lexical material into the phrase marker being constructed with the fewest nodes consistent with the well-formedness rules of the language
New cards
75
Kirkham et al. (2002); Fiser & Aslin (2002)
People are about as successful with learning the "words" when it is a visual stimuli, rather than an auditory one. Not just applicable to language.
New cards
76
Marcus et al (1999)
9-month-olds can only learn when AAB patterns are varied. Children need variation not repetition to learn abstract patterns.
New cards
77
Non-adjacent dependencies
Children track non-adjacent co-occurrence probabilities only if adjacent transitions are highly unpredictable.
New cards
78
Gomez (2002)
Infants can pick out frames that are distributionally like functional elements. Distributional information is key to category learning more generally.
New cards
79
Braine (1987)
Tested adults' ability to learn MN/PQ systems under different conditions. Adding a meaning cue helps to solve the MN/PQ problem. Many natural languages have semantic cues to noun categories (grammatical gender/noun class systems)
New cards
80
Gerken et al. (2005)
Tested children's ability to learn a complex gender system without semantic cues. Children can learn a complex morphological pattern based on phonological cues, but they have to be substantial cues. Semantic cues aren't necessary but some additional distributional cue must be present. If cues are around, then infants can learn grammatical categories through statistical learning.
New cards
81
Reeder et al. (2013)
Tested adult's generalisation of words to new contexts under different conditions. If they hear all A&B, or there's partial overlap, they learn the system. Partial overlap, 3x Exposure, they only learn heard forms.
New cards
82
Saffran and Thiessen (2003)
Tested 9-month-old infants' ability to learn a novel phonotactic constraint (voiceless stops in onset and voiced stops in coda, and vice versa). Phonotactic rules with fewer features are easier to learn.
New cards
83
Culbertson & Newport (2015)
Tested 4- to 7-year-olds' ability to learn a novel syntactic rule. Simpler syntactic rules are easier to learn.
New cards
84
Hudson Kam & Newport (2009)
Test whether children reduce unconditioned variation by regularising. They do.
New cards
85
Soderstrom (2007)
Properties of IDS: diminutives, lower type:token ratio, lower MLU, higher, more variable pitch, exaggeration of prosodic cues, hyperarticulation of sounds.
New cards
86
Newport et al. (1977)
Properties of CDS: Short MLU, well-formed, structurally simple, lots of deixis, lots of repetitions
New cards
87
Hurtado et al. (2008)
More maternal (and paternal) input at 18mo = larger vocabulary size at 24mo
New cards
88
Specific disorders
There is a deficit in one or a small number of skills, with typical functioning in other areas, e.g., SLI/DLD
New cards
89
General disorders
Impairments in most, if not all, cognitive functions, e.g. Down syndrome
New cards
90
Normal IQ
85-115
New cards
91
Moderate learning difficulties IQ
50-70
New cards
92
Severe learning difficulties IQ
New cards
93
Bishop (2016/17)
There are a group of children who have difficulty learning language not caused by autism, general mental delay, severe emotional disorder, acquired childhood aphasia, or peripheral impairments. They have DLD.
New cards
94
Developmental Language Disorder
A developmental disorder affecting primarily oral language, more specifically grammatical abilities. Affects 7% of school population. Asymmetry between cognitive and language abilities
New cards
95
Exclusion criteria for SLI
Normal hearing, no known history of recurrent otitis media, no emotional or behavioural problems, performance IQ 85 or above, normal neurological status.
New cards
96
Language profile for DLD
Omits grammatical morphemes or use wrong ones, difficulty producing or comprehending complex syntactic structures, difficulty producing cohesive narratives.
New cards
97
Rice & Wexler (2001)
Development of English tense in children with DLD is much slower than their TD peers.
New cards
98
van der Lely & Battell (2003)
Wh-questions in DLD children. No do-support, and gap-filling.
New cards
99
Gathercole & Baddeley (1990); Orsolini et al (2001)
DLD children have problems with phonological processing.
New cards
100
Kail (1994); Montgomery & Leonard (1998); Tallal et al. (1996)
DLD children have longer latencies when performing both language-mediated and non-language-mediated activities