Newborns prefer to listen to speech over non-speech
2
New cards
Vouloumanos et al (2010)
Preference for human speech over primate vocalisation appears after 3 months
3
New cards
Ramus et al. (2000)
Newborns can discriminate languages but not when the recording is backwards
4
New cards
Jusczyk, Cutler & Redanz (1993)
American 9-month-olds prefer to listen to low-pass filtered strong/weak words over weak/strong words
5
New cards
Jusczyk et al. (1993)
American and dutch 9-month-olds listen longer to their native words that feature language-specific segmental phonotactics - but not if low-pass filtered.
6
New cards
Jusczyk, Luce & Charles-Luce (1994)
English-learning 9-month-olds listen longer to nonsense syllables with high phonotactic probabilities
7
New cards
Swingley & Aslin (2002)
14-month-olds look less at the corresponding picture if the word is 'mispronounced' by a single feature
8
New cards
Maye, Werker, & Gerken (2002)
6-month-olds can perform distributional learning from phonetic input
9
New cards
Aitchison & Chiat (1981)
4- to 9-year-olds produce 'consonant harmony' in recall of newly learned words. However, more likely to be a failure of memory rather than articulation.
10
New cards
Brent & Siskind (2001)
Only a small portion of utterances in infant-directed speech consist of a single word
11
New cards
Saffran, Aslin, & Newport (1996)
Infants looked longer at the side that was playing part words. Novelty effect.
12
New cards
Cutler & Carter (1987)
The most common prosodic word type in English is strong-weak disyllables.
13
New cards
Kelly & Martin (1994)
Strong syllable = word onset 95% of the time in IDS
14
New cards
Jusczyk, Houston & Newsome (1999)
7.5-month-old infants use predominant prosodic patterns to segment words
15
New cards
Bortfeld et al. (2005)
Infants can use familiar words for further segmentation
16
New cards
Casello et al. (1999)
Noun bias in early lexical development, possibly due to the whole object assumption
17
New cards
Landau, Smith & Jones (1988)
Shape bias makes children extend the referent of a noun based on its shape
18
New cards
Smith and Yu (2008)
Children can use co-occurrence probabilities to perform cross-situational learning
19
New cards
Tomasello & Barton (1994)
2-year-olds can determine the referent of an unknown word based on their understanding of speaker intention (theory of mind)
20
New cards
Akhtar et al. (1996)
2-year-olds can also determine the referent of an unknown word based on their understanding of the speaker's knowledge state.
21
New cards
Markman (1990)
Mechanisms behind word-learning are specific to language. Evidence: shape bias only observed when words are involved.
22
New cards
Bloom (2000)
Mechanisms behind word-learning apply generally. Evidence: children can fast-map word-meaning as well as facts. Mutual exclusivity works both for word-learning and fact-learning.
23
New cards
Dual Route Model
All inflected forms learnt through memory. The child over applies a default rule to irregulars. Then irregular forms are learned and block rule application.
24
New cards
Single Route Model
Stores some past-tense forms in memory. Forms with infrequent patterns adopt similar patterns due to analogy. The child recovers from over-regularisation through reinforced memory of irregular patterns.
25
New cards
Rumelhart & McLelland (1986)
Fed a computer verbs (10 high frequency for 10 trials, 410 medium for 190). Then tested network on present tenses and saw what past-tense form it supplied.
26
New cards
Marcus et al. (1992)
The emergence of -ed marking is sudden, and coincides with over-regularisation. This shows that -ed is acquired as a rule (which overapplies)
27
New cards
Marchman (1997)
Regular verbs are not entirely protected from frequency effects. Children sometimes over-irregularise when there are many similar-sounding irregulars.
28
New cards
Clahsen & Almazan (1998)
Children with WS show deficiency in irregular past-tense verbs, but not regular ones. Rule and memory are different components.
29
New cards
Gopnik & Crago (1991)
Children with SLI show deficiency in regular past-tense verbs but not in irregular ones. Rule and memory are different components.
30
New cards
McClelland & Patterson (2002)
A phonological deficit can explain some of the dissociation (consonant clusters). SR Model
31
New cards
Gerken & McIntosh (1993)
Children can find the correct picture better when they are told "Find the dog for me" than "Find was dog for me"
32
New cards
Dye et al. (2018)
10-month-olds prefer "the dog" over "dog the"
33
New cards
Constructivist accounts
Syntactic knowledge develops gradually from concrete item-based learning to abstract structural knowledge.
34
New cards
Generative approach
Abstract categories are innate (includes lexical and functional). Children are aware early on of the relationship between functional and lexical categories.
35
New cards
Pine and Lieven (1997)
No overlap in the use of DET with the same nouns (item-based)
36
New cards
Valian et al (2009)
Children's overlap in DET use is about 50% (same as adults)
37
New cards
Yang (2010)
Cannot expect high levels of overlap in spontaneous speech because we don't always recombine articles for every noun.
38
New cards
Semantic bootstrapping
Meaning-category correspondence guides the initial phase of category and relationship learning in syntax.
39
New cards
Pinker (1984)
Children are innately equipped with knowledge of the mapping between syntactic category types and functions on one hand and semantic types on the other.
40
New cards
Phonological/prosodic bootstrapping
Phonological and prosodic cues aid the learning of syntactic categories and phrases. Syntactic boundaries are marked by prosodic boundaries.
41
New cards
Shi et al. (1998)
Function words tend to have shorter vowel durations, weaker amplitude, and simpler syllable structure than lexical words.
42
New cards
Kelly (1988)
In English, disyllabic nouns tend to have initial stress while disyllabic verbs tend to have final stress.
43
New cards
Distributional learning
A word's syntactic category and its grammatical role in a sentence can be learned by looking at the words that surround it, its morphological marking, and its relative position in a sentence. It goes beyond the frequency of occurrence of a particular word.
44
New cards
Wexler (1998)
Very Early Parameter Setting. Head directionality is set very early.
45
New cards
Akhtar & Tomasello (1997)
2- to 3-year-olds' verb use closely parallels what they have heard. Even if that means accepting word order configurations inconsistent with the target language
46
New cards
Gertner et al. (2006)
Even 21-month-olds performed above chance with novel verbs in preferential looking procedure
47
New cards
Fernandes et al. (2006)
28- and 34-month-olds can generalise from neutral and intransitive sentences.
48
New cards
Savage et al. (2003)
Children's structural priming seems to be lexically based (at least before age 4)
49
New cards
Radford (1996)
Children's early grammars are truncated versions of the adult grammar (small clauses)
50
New cards
Rizzi (1993/4)
Children project all the necessary functional categories available in the adult grammar but children do not necessarily know the obligatoriness of these categories (Truncation Account)
51
New cards
ATOM: Wexler (1998)
Agreement/Tense Omission Model. Children's early grammars project all functional categories, parameters are set from the earliest stages. Children optionally produce them due to a maturational constraint.
52
New cards
Unique Checking Constraint (UCC)
Check the features of a verb against only one functional category -> an item can be checked at AGR or TNS but not both.
53
New cards
MOSAIC: Freudenthal et al. (2007)
Model of syntax acquisition in children. Empiricist/Usage-based approaches: Children learn strings of language directly from the input.
54
New cards
Freudenthal et al. (2007)
Recency effect: Good correlation between utterance-final non-finites in the input and RIs in children.
55
New cards
Legate & Yang (2007)
Variational model. Combines presence of categories (=/-TNS) with nature of the input. Utterances with (overtly marked) tense/agreement reward + TNS. Captures crosslinguistic data by combining categories with input data.
56
New cards
Nativism (G)
Most core aspects of syntactic knowledge are part of Universal Grammar and innate
57
New cards
Development (G)
Involves selecting the options set by UG and learning language-specific properties of syntax outside UG
58
New cards
Continuity (G)
What underlies children's sentence is the same kind of abstract grammar that adults have.
59
New cards
Non-adultlike production/comprehension (G)
Either they haven't set UG to the correct settings, they are still learning idiosyncratic properties of the language, or committing performance errors.
60
New cards
Emergentism (C)
Much of syntactic knowledge is learned from the input
61
New cards
Development (C)
Involves learning concrete exemplars of word combinations, and then gradually making generalisations over such exemplars.
62
New cards
Discontinuity (C)
What underlies young children's sentence is item-based and not the kind of abstract grammar that adults have.
63
New cards
Non-adultlike production.comprehension (C)
Because children start by learning fixed combinations, they sometimes overapply them to inappropriate contexts.
64
New cards
Goodluck & Tavakolian (1982)
Children have adultlike syntactic knowledge but are not good at processing sentences in experimental tasks. (G)
65
New cards
Diessel & Tomasello (2000)
Children's early relative clauses are just amalgams of a prefabricated schema and a bare untensed VP. (C)
66
New cards
Kidd et al. (2007)
Children are better at processing types of relative clauses that they hear or say the most (C)
67
New cards
Santelman et al. (2002)
Do-support is an idiosyncratic feature of English (outside UG) that just needs to be learned (G)
68
New cards
Guasti et al. (1995)
English-learning children mis-set the negation parameter to the 'do not raise' setting (G)
69
New cards
Rowland & Pine (2000)
Children are accurate in production of items that are frequent in the input, and inaccurate for those that aren't
70
New cards
Ambridge & Rowland (2009)
Children who have have the 'What does [THING] [PROCESS]" schema produce doubling errors, children who say "What she does like?", don't. (C)
71
New cards
Trueswell & Gleitman (2007)
Children become sensitive to phrasal ordering predictors (word order), lexcially specific predictors, and less reliable semantic, contextual and referential predictors.
72
New cards
Trueswell et al. (1999)
Put the frog on the napkin in the box. The Kindergarten Path Effect.
73
New cards
Late closure
When possible, attach incoming lexical items into the clause or phrase currently being processed
74
New cards
Minimal attachment
Attach incoming lexical material into the phrase marker being constructed with the fewest nodes consistent with the well-formedness rules of the language
75
New cards
Kirkham et al. (2002); Fiser & Aslin (2002)
People are about as successful with learning the "words" when it is a visual stimuli, rather than an auditory one. Not just applicable to language.
76
New cards
Marcus et al (1999)
9-month-olds can only learn when AAB patterns are varied. Children need variation not repetition to learn abstract patterns.
77
New cards
Non-adjacent dependencies
Children track non-adjacent co-occurrence probabilities only if adjacent transitions are highly unpredictable.
78
New cards
Gomez (2002)
Infants can pick out frames that are distributionally like functional elements. Distributional information is key to category learning more generally.
79
New cards
Braine (1987)
Tested adults' ability to learn MN/PQ systems under different conditions. Adding a meaning cue helps to solve the MN/PQ problem. Many natural languages have semantic cues to noun categories (grammatical gender/noun class systems)
80
New cards
Gerken et al. (2005)
Tested children's ability to learn a complex gender system without semantic cues. Children can learn a complex morphological pattern based on phonological cues, but they have to be substantial cues. Semantic cues aren't necessary but some additional distributional cue must be present. If cues are around, then infants can learn grammatical categories through statistical learning.
81
New cards
Reeder et al. (2013)
Tested adult's generalisation of words to new contexts under different conditions. If they hear all A&B, or there's partial overlap, they learn the system. Partial overlap, 3x Exposure, they only learn heard forms.
82
New cards
Saffran and Thiessen (2003)
Tested 9-month-old infants' ability to learn a novel phonotactic constraint (voiceless stops in onset and voiced stops in coda, and vice versa). Phonotactic rules with fewer features are easier to learn.
83
New cards
Culbertson & Newport (2015)
Tested 4- to 7-year-olds' ability to learn a novel syntactic rule. Simpler syntactic rules are easier to learn.
84
New cards
Hudson Kam & Newport (2009)
Test whether children reduce unconditioned variation by regularising. They do.
85
New cards
Soderstrom (2007)
Properties of IDS: diminutives, lower type:token ratio, lower MLU, higher, more variable pitch, exaggeration of prosodic cues, hyperarticulation of sounds.
86
New cards
Newport et al. (1977)
Properties of CDS: Short MLU, well-formed, structurally simple, lots of deixis, lots of repetitions
87
New cards
Hurtado et al. (2008)
More maternal (and paternal) input at 18mo = larger vocabulary size at 24mo
88
New cards
Specific disorders
There is a deficit in one or a small number of skills, with typical functioning in other areas, e.g., SLI/DLD
89
New cards
General disorders
Impairments in most, if not all, cognitive functions, e.g. Down syndrome
90
New cards
Normal IQ
85-115
91
New cards
Moderate learning difficulties IQ
50-70
92
New cards
Severe learning difficulties IQ
93
New cards
Bishop (2016/17)
There are a group of children who have difficulty learning language not caused by autism, general mental delay, severe emotional disorder, acquired childhood aphasia, or peripheral impairments. They have DLD.
94
New cards
Developmental Language Disorder
A developmental disorder affecting primarily oral language, more specifically grammatical abilities. Affects 7% of school population. Asymmetry between cognitive and language abilities
95
New cards
Exclusion criteria for SLI
Normal hearing, no known history of recurrent otitis media, no emotional or behavioural problems, performance IQ 85 or above, normal neurological status.
96
New cards
Language profile for DLD
Omits grammatical morphemes or use wrong ones, difficulty producing or comprehending complex syntactic structures, difficulty producing cohesive narratives.
97
New cards
Rice & Wexler (2001)
Development of English tense in children with DLD is much slower than their TD peers.
98
New cards
van der Lely & Battell (2003)
Wh-questions in DLD children. No do-support, and gap-filling.
99
New cards
Gathercole & Baddeley (1990); Orsolini et al (2001)
DLD children have problems with phonological processing.
100
New cards
Kail (1994); Montgomery & Leonard (1998); Tallal et al. (1996)
DLD children have longer latencies when performing both language-mediated and non-language-mediated activities