Source: Barron's AP Psychology
Translation of stimuli into neural impulses
These impulses first travel to the thalamus, then onto different cortices
Reflected light first enters through the cornea (a protective covering) that focuses the light
Light goes through the pupil
Muscles that control the pupil (iris) to let more or less light in
Accommodation: light that enters the pupil is focused by the lens. As light passes through the lens, the image is inverted.
The focused inverted image projects on the retina which has specialized neurons that are activated by the different wavelengths of light
First layer of cells are directly activated by light (rods and cones)
If enough rods and cones fire in an area of the retina, they activate the next layer of bipolar cells.
If enough bipolar cells fire, the next layer, ganglion cells, are activated
The axons of the ganglion cells make up the optic nerve that sends these impulses to the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), which is an area of the thalamus
From there, the messages are sent to the visual cortices located in the occipital lobes of the brain
Discovered that groups of neurons in the visual cortex respond to different types of visual images.
Feature detectors for vertical lines, curves, motion, etc.
3 types of cones in the retina: cones that detect blue, red, and green
Cones are activated in different combinations to produce all the colours of the visible spectrum
Cannot explain some visual phenomena
Sensory receptors arranged in the retina come in pairs
Red/Green, Yellow/Blue, Black/White
If one sensor is stimulated, its pair is inhibited from firing
Can explain visual phenomena
If you stare at one colour for a while and then look at a white or blank space, you will see a colour afterimage.
If you stare at green, the afterimage is red. If you stare at blue, the afterimage is yellow.
Dichromatic: cannot see either red/green or blue/yellow shades.
Monochromatic: can only see shades of grey
Length of the waves and determines the pitch (megahertz)
High-pitched sounds have high frequencies, and the waves are densely packed together.
Low-pitched sounds have low frequencies, and the waves are spaced apart.
Sound waves are collected in your outer ear (pinna) and travel down the ear canal until they reach the eardrum (tympanic membrane).
The membrane vibrates as the sound waves hit it and is attached to the first in a series of 3 small bones (collectively known as the ossicles)
Eardrum connects with the hammer (malleus), which connects to the anvil (incus), which connects to the stirrup (stapes)
The vibration of the eardrum is transmitted by these 3 bones to the oval window which is attached the to cochlea, a structure shaped like a snail’s shell filled with fluid. As the oval window vibrates, the fluid moves.
The floor of the cochlea is called the basilar membrane. It’s lined with hair cells connected to the organ of Corti, which are neurons activated by the movement of the hair cells. When the fluid moves, the hair cells move and transduction occurs. The organ of Corti fires, and these impulses are transmitted to the brain via the auditory nerve.
Hair cells in the cochlea respond to different frequencies of sound based on where they are located in the cochlea.
Some bend in response to high pitches and some to low.
We sense pitch because the hair cells move in different places in the cochlea
Place theory accurately describes how hair cells sense the upper range of pitches, but not the lower tones.
Lower tones are sensed by the rate at which the cells fire.
We sense pitch because the hair cells fire at different rates (frequencies) in the cochlea.
Occurs when the hair cells in the cochlea are damaged, usually by loud noise.
Prolonged exposure to loud noise can permanently damage the hair cells in your cochlea, and these hair cells do not regenerate.
Some pain messages have a higher priority than others.
When a higher priority message is sent, the gate swings open for it and swings shut for a low priority message, which we will not feel.
Endorphins (pain-killing chemicals) also swing the gate shut
Molecules settle in a mucous membrane at the top of each nostril and are absorbed by receptor cells located there.
Receptor cells are linked to the olfactory bulb, which gathers the messages from the olfactory receptor cells and sends this info to the brain.
Tells us about how our body is oriented in space.
3 semicircular canals filled with fluid in the inner ear give the brain feedback about body orientation
When position of your head changes, the fluid moves in the canals, causing sensors in the canals to move.
The movement of these hair cells activate neurons and their impulses go to the brain.
Tells us about the position and orientation of specific body parts.
Receptors in our muscles and joints send info to our brain about our limbs.
This info, along with visual feedback, lets us keep track of our body.
Computes the difference threshold
States that the change needed is proportional to the original intensity of the stimulus. The more intense the stimulus, the more it will need to change before we notice a difference.
Each sense varies according to a constant, but the constants differ between the senses.
Constant for hearing is 5%, constant for vision is 8%
Investigates the effects of the distractions and interference we experience while perceiving the world.
Tries to predict what we will perceive among competing stimuli
Takes into account how motivated we are to detect certain stimuli and what we expect to perceive
All the above factors are called the response criteria
We perceive by filling in gaps in what we sense.
Occurs when you use your background knowledge to fill in gaps in what you perceive
We use only the features of the object itself to build a complete perception.
Feature detectors in the visual cortex allow us to perceive basic features, such as lines, curves, motions, etc
Our mind builds the picture from the bottom up using these basic characteristics
Images in a series of still pictures presented at a certain speed will appear to be moving.
Examples: movies, flip books
A series of lightbulbs turned on and off at a particular rate will appear to be one moving light.
Examples: movie marquees, holiday lights
Used to determine when human infants can perceive depth
Infant is placed onto one side of a glass-topped table that creates the impression of a cliff.
Found that an infant old enough to crawl will not crawl across the visual cliff, implying the child has depth perception.
Depth cues that do not depend on having two eyes
Linear perspective, relative size cue, Interposition cue, texture gradient, shadowing
As an object gets closer to our face, our eyes must move toward each other to keep focused on the object.
The more the eyes converge, the closer the object must be.