1/6
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
temperament of the child
Kagan 1984 suggest that temperament of the child is actually what leads to different attachment types, children with different innate temperaments will have different attachment types
Supported by Fox 1989 who found that babies with an 'easy' (eat, sleep regularly and accept new experiences) temperament are likely to develop secure attachments
Babies with 'slow to warm up' (take while to get used to new experiences) temperament are more likely to have insecure-avoidant attachments
Babies with 'difficult' (eat and sleep irregularly and reject new experiences) temperament are likely to be insecure-resistant
Belsky and Rovine 1987 propose interactionist theory to explain different attachment types as they say that child's attachment type is result of innate temperament and how parents respond to them
Van Ijzendoorn and Kroonenberg 1988
understand variations in attachment in different cultures to see if the three main attachment types could be applied universally (and if there were differences within cultures)
And find the proportions of each type
using method called meta-analysis (32 studies from 8 countries, 1990 children)
Studies all included
Strange situation used
Observed only mother-infant pairs
Classified infants into one of the attachment types A (avoidant), B (secure), C (ambivalent or resistant)
Choice of studies excluded any identifying special groups of children, such as those with Down Syndrome, any less than 35 pairs and any using children older than 2 years
Calculated averages of individuals who were classified as having secure, insecure-avoidant or insecure-resistant attachment in each country
results
Stacked bar chart
Secure attachment was most common in all countries
Higher number of insecure-avoidant attachments in individualist cultures rather than collectivist
Developing independence, less clingy
Higher number of insecure-resistant attachments in collectivist cultures rather than individualist
Angry that they are left with stranger as they will typically be very well introduced to neighbours and community so unused to strangers
Variations of results within countries was 150% higher than variations between countries
Suggests that different environments (urban v rural) affect parenting within countries
conclusion
Modest cross-cultural differences reflect the effects of mass media, which portrays similar notions of parenting
Overall consistency in secure attachment types leads to the conclusion that there may be universal/innate characteristics that underpin infant and caregiver interactions
Significant variations of insecure attachments demonstrate that universality is limited, implications include linking of the variation in attachment to child-rearing practices and environmental factors
German study highlights high percentage of avoidant behaviour, typical of independent children
Grossmann et al 1985 says that German parents seek 'independent, non-clingy infants, who do not make demands on parents, but obey their commands'
Israeli children were reared in communal living, so were used to separation from their mother and showed no anxiety when this occurs
Not used to strangers so were distressed and this explains the high percentage of resistant behaviour
Individualist - rates of insecure-resistant similar to Ainsworth's whereas in collectivist is was higher
Secure attachment seems to be the norm in most cultures, supporting Bowlby's idea that attachment is innate and universal however cultural practices also have influence on attachment type
strength
Research conducted by indigenous psychologists who have same background as the ppts (enhances the validity of research as miscommunication due to language barriers are limited) - Grossman is German and Takahashi is Japanese
Difficulties also include bias as one nation's stereotypes may override findings but the indigenous psychologists helped limit this
HOWEVER, this is not true for all cross-cultural attachment research as Morelli and Tronick 1991 were outsiders from America when they studied the child-rearing and patterns of attachment in the Efé of Zaire
Their data may have been affected by difficulties in gathering data outside their own culture - leads to bias and lack of cross-cultural communication
Standardised procedure used
Comparison is easier to make as all studies used the Strange Situation
Classification is the same for attachment styles
Wide array of countries used - not all Western
Truly cross-cultural research
High population validity
weakness
Findings from each country may not be representative of cultural variation within the country
150% higher variation within countries
Disproportionately high number of studies done in the USA (15/32)
Also only 8 countries which is just not representative globally
Could be confounding variables that are not considered when analysing research (ex: sample differences in SES, urban/rural backgrounds, and situational variables when conducting the study)
Methodology is not matched, sample characteristics such as poverty, social class, urban/rural make-up can confound results as can age of participants
Environmental variables may differ between studies and confound results - babies may appear to explore more in studies conducted in small rooms
Less visible proximity-seeking because of room size may make it likely that the child is labelled avoidant
Imposed etic: when we assume an idea or technique that works in one cultural context will work in another.
Emic is cultural uniqueness whereas etic is cross-cultural universality
Behaviours measured by the Strange Situation may not have the same meanings in different cultural contexts and comparing them across cultures is meaningless - in UK a lack of affection in reunion is seen as avoidant but in Germany this would be independence
Similar attachment types in countries would suggest that attachment is innate and universal so supports Bowlby's theory
HOWEVER, global media represents a particular view of how parents and babies are meant to behave which may override traditional cultural differences in the way children are brought up
additional studies
Italian study
Simonelli et al. (2014) assessed 76 1-year old babies using the Strange Situation. Wanted to see whether the proportions matched those found in previous studies
50% secure, 36% insecure-avoidant, which is a higher proportion of avoidant attachment compared to other studies
Increasing numbers of mothers working long hours and prefer professional childcare and these results are reflective of cultural changes.
Patterns of attachment types are not static but vary in line with cultural change
South Korea study
Jin et al. (2012) used Strange Situati
on to assess 87 babies. Compared proportions to other studies.
Overall proportions of attachment types similar to other countries, however only one baby was classed as insecure avoidant and many were insecure-resistant
Distribution is similar to Japan as they have similar child-rearing styles
Japan study
Takahashi (1990) used Strange Situation for 60 infants. Higher levels of infant anxiety when left alone than other studies.
0% insecure-avoidant, 68% secure, 32% insecure-resistant