cultural variations of SS and attachment styles

0.0(0)
studied byStudied by 0 people
learnLearn
examPractice Test
spaced repetitionSpaced Repetition
heart puzzleMatch
flashcardsFlashcards
Card Sorting

1/6

encourage image

There's no tags or description

Looks like no tags are added yet.

Study Analytics
Name
Mastery
Learn
Test
Matching
Spaced

No study sessions yet.

7 Terms

1
New cards

temperament of the child

  • Kagan 1984 suggest that temperament of the child is actually what leads to different attachment types, children with different innate temperaments will have different attachment types

  • Supported by Fox 1989 who found that babies with an 'easy' (eat, sleep regularly and accept new experiences) temperament are likely to develop secure attachments

  • Babies with 'slow to warm up' (take while to get used to new experiences) temperament are more likely to have insecure-avoidant attachments

  • Babies with 'difficult' (eat and sleep irregularly and reject new experiences) temperament are likely to be insecure-resistant

 

  • Belsky and Rovine 1987 propose interactionist theory to explain different attachment types as they say that child's attachment type is result of innate temperament and how parents respond to them

2
New cards

Van Ijzendoorn and Kroonenberg 1988

  • understand variations in attachment in different cultures to see if the three main attachment types could be applied universally (and if there were differences within cultures)

    • And find the proportions of each type

  • using method called meta-analysis (32 studies from 8 countries, 1990 children)

  • Studies all included

    • Strange situation used

    • Observed only mother-infant pairs

    • Classified infants into one of the attachment types A (avoidant), B (secure), C (ambivalent or resistant)

  • Choice of studies excluded any identifying special groups of children, such as those with Down Syndrome, any less than 35 pairs and any using children older than 2 years

  • Calculated averages of individuals who were classified as having secure, insecure-avoidant or insecure-resistant attachment in each country

3
New cards

results

  • Stacked bar chart

  • Secure attachment was most common in all countries

  • Higher number of insecure-avoidant attachments in individualist cultures rather than collectivist

    • Developing independence, less clingy

  • Higher number of insecure-resistant attachments in collectivist cultures rather than individualist

    • Angry that they are left with stranger as they will typically be very well introduced to neighbours and community so unused to strangers

  • Variations of results within countries was 150% higher than variations between countries

    • Suggests that different environments (urban v rural) affect parenting within countries

<ul><li><p><span>Stacked bar chart</span></p></li><li><p><span>Secure attachment was most common in all countries</span></p></li><li><p><span>Higher number of insecure-avoidant attachments in individualist cultures rather than collectivist</span></p><ul><li><p><span>Developing independence, less clingy</span></p></li></ul></li><li><p><span>Higher number of insecure-resistant attachments in collectivist cultures rather than individualist</span></p><ul><li><p><span>Angry that they are left with stranger as they will typically be very well introduced to neighbours and community so unused to strangers</span></p></li></ul></li><li><p><span>Variations of results within countries was 150% higher than variations between countries</span></p><ul><li><p><span>Suggests that different environments (urban v rural) affect parenting within countries</span></p></li></ul></li></ul><p></p>
4
New cards

conclusion

  • Modest cross-cultural differences reflect the effects of mass media, which portrays similar notions of parenting

  • Overall consistency in secure attachment types leads to the conclusion that there may be universal/innate characteristics that underpin infant and caregiver interactions

  • Significant variations of insecure attachments demonstrate that universality is limited, implications include linking of the variation in attachment to child-rearing practices and environmental factors

  • German study highlights high percentage of avoidant behaviour, typical of independent children

    • Grossmann et al 1985 says that German parents seek 'independent, non-clingy infants, who do not make demands on parents, but obey their commands'

  • Israeli children were reared in communal living, so were used to separation from their mother and showed no anxiety when this occurs

    • Not used to strangers so were distressed and this explains the high percentage of resistant behaviour

 

  • Individualist - rates of insecure-resistant similar to Ainsworth's whereas in collectivist is was higher

  • Secure attachment seems to be the norm in most cultures, supporting Bowlby's idea that attachment is innate and universal however cultural practices also have influence on attachment type

5
New cards

strength

  • Research conducted by indigenous psychologists who have same background as the ppts (enhances the validity of research as miscommunication due to language barriers are limited) - Grossman is German and Takahashi is Japanese

  • Difficulties also include bias as one nation's stereotypes may override findings but the indigenous psychologists helped limit this

    • HOWEVER, this is not true for all cross-cultural attachment research as Morelli and Tronick 1991 were outsiders from America when they studied the child-rearing and patterns of attachment in the Efé of Zaire

    • Their data may have been affected by difficulties in gathering data outside their own culture - leads to bias and lack of cross-cultural communication

  • Standardised procedure used

    • Comparison is easier to make as all studies used the Strange Situation

    • Classification is the same for attachment styles

  • Wide array of countries used - not all Western

    • Truly cross-cultural research

    • High population validity

6
New cards

weakness

  • Findings from each country may not be representative of cultural variation within the country

    • 150% higher variation within countries

  • Disproportionately high number of studies done in the USA (15/32)

    • Also only 8 countries which is just not representative globally

  • Could be confounding variables that are not considered when analysing research (ex: sample differences in SES, urban/rural backgrounds, and situational variables when conducting the study)

    • Methodology is not matched, sample characteristics such as poverty, social class, urban/rural make-up can confound results as can age of participants

    • Environmental variables may differ between studies and confound results - babies may appear to explore more in studies conducted in small rooms

    • Less visible proximity-seeking because of room size may make it likely that the child is labelled avoidant

  • Imposed etic: when we assume an idea or technique that works in one cultural context will work in another.

    • Emic is cultural uniqueness whereas etic is cross-cultural universality

    • Behaviours measured by the Strange Situation may not have the same meanings in different cultural contexts and comparing them across cultures is meaningless - in UK a lack of affection in reunion is seen as avoidant but in Germany this would be independence

  • Similar attachment types in countries would suggest that attachment is innate and universal so supports Bowlby's theory

    • HOWEVER, global media represents a particular view of how parents and babies are meant to behave which may override traditional cultural differences in the way children are brought up

7
New cards

additional studies

  • Italian study

  • Simonelli et al. (2014) assessed 76 1-year old babies using the Strange Situation. Wanted to see whether the proportions matched those found in previous studies

  • 50% secure, 36% insecure-avoidant, which is a higher proportion of avoidant attachment compared to other studies

  • Increasing numbers of mothers working long hours and prefer professional childcare and these results are reflective of cultural changes.

  • Patterns of attachment types are not static but vary in line with cultural change

 

  • South Korea study

  • Jin et al. (2012) used Strange Situati

    on to assess 87 babies. Compared proportions to other studies.

  • Overall proportions of attachment types similar to other countries, however only one baby was classed as insecure avoidant and many were insecure-resistant

  • Distribution is similar to Japan as they have similar child-rearing styles

 

  • Japan study

  • Takahashi (1990) used Strange Situation for 60 infants. Higher levels of infant anxiety when left alone than other studies.

  • 0% insecure-avoidant, 68% secure, 32% insecure-resistant