FInal Exam Cog and Lang Development

studied byStudied by 1 person
0.0(0)
learn
LearnA personalized and smart learning plan
exam
Practice TestTake a test on your terms and definitions
spaced repetition
Spaced RepetitionScientifically backed study method
heart puzzle
Matching GameHow quick can you match all your cards?
flashcards
FlashcardsStudy terms and definitions

1 / 116

flashcard set

Earn XP

Description and Tags

117 Terms

1
Words
  • are symbols: stand for something without being part of it.

  • are arbitrary: the sounds /dog/ mean "dog" but there is nothing dog-like about the sounds

  • have reference: stand for their referents, not just accompany them

New cards
2
Context-bound words
words children only say in a specific context
New cards
3
Are children's first words all context-bound words? Or are some first words already referential?
  • longitudinal, observational study of 4 English-learning infants

  • recorded all language production

  • examined whether first 10 words were context-bound or not

  • results: children's first 10 words included both context-bound and referential words

  • role of input: the "context" of the context-bound words was the mother's most frequent usage

New cards
4
The first 50 words
  • acquired around 18 months

  • of first 50 words, 45% are nouns and only 3% are verbs

New cards
5
Natural Partitions Hypothesis
  • meanings linked to verbs are harder to learn because

  1. nouns typically refer to objects, which are easy to perceive and are stable in the environment

  2. verbs typically refer to actions that are transient and relational

New cards
6
Theory: different learning mechanisms in acquisition of nouns and verbs
  • the meanings of nouns can be learned by mapping sounds onto the nonlinguistic context

  • the meanings of verbs often require a syntactic frame that identifies the nature of the relationship involved

New cards
7
Do all children show the noun bias, cross-linguistically and cross-culturally?
  • 8- to 16-month-olds learning English, Cantonese, or Mandarin

  • parents completed a checklist of words their child could produce

  • results: object nouns and action words were more similar in frequency in Cantonese and Mandarin than in English

  • role of parental input and syntax in acquisition of nouns among English learners

New cards
8
Underextensions
when children use a word in fewer contexts than they should ("dog" to refer only to collies)
New cards
9
overextensions
when children use a word in more contexts than they should ("Daddy" to refer to all adult men)
New cards
10
Why do children overextend word meanings?
  • experimental study with 2-year-olds

  • children named three pictures (priming) and then a target object

  • one picture was perceptually similar to the target object

  • results: priming led to higher rates of overextension of the primed word

New cards
11
Word Spurt
after children acquire 50 words (around 18 months), they begin acquiring words at a much faster rate
New cards
12
Is the Word Spurt real?
  • some children may show spurts, but these are highly individualistic and may depend on specific experiences

  • word spurt may not happen at the same time for every child

New cards
13
Word comprehension
- children can understand much more than they can say
- children's word comprehension can begin as young as 5 months
New cards
14
Measuring infants' word comprehension
  • tested 6-9-month-old infants

  • parents wore a visor and headphones and repeated sentences that they heard (ex. "Can you find the banana?")

  • results: even the youngest children (ages 6-7 months) looked longer at the labeled picture, indicating word comprehension

New cards
15
follow-up study with children ages 6-20 months
- results: all ages were above chance at looking at target picture, but there was a big improvement between 13-14 months
New cards
16
Referential style
more nouns and fewer social expressions
New cards
17
Expressive style
more social expressions and fewer nouns
New cards
18
Sources of the referential/expressive difference
  • mothers who spend more time teaching labels for objects tend to have more referential children

  • first-born children of college-educated parents are more likely to be referential

  • children may simply differ in style

New cards
19
Larger vocabularies are found among children who...
  • hear more speech from adults

  • hear more variation in words and kinds of sentences

  • hear more decontextualized language (talk about the past and future, pretend play, and definitions)

  • hear words in more elaborative contexts

  • hear speech that is responsive to their own behavior (especially for infants under 18 months)

  • are first-born

  • are of high SES household

New cards
20
Word segmentation
ability to break stream of speech sounds into distinct words
New cards
21
Transitional probabilities in speech sounds
  • speech stream contained 3-syllable "words" that were only distinguished by their transitional probabilities

  • 8-month-olds discriminated words versus part-words after 2 minutes of listening

New cards
22
Are "chunks" really words for the child? Or just a sound sequence?
8-month-olds discriminate "words" from non-words in an English-language context but not a nonsense context
New cards
23
Stress as a cue to word segmentation
  • in English, stress is usually on the first syllable

  • infants can use stress as a cue to word boundaries as early as 9 months

New cards
24
What happens if transitional probabilities and stress cues do not coincide?
when pitted against each other, 8-month-old English-language learners used stress cues instead of transitional probabilities
New cards
25
The whole-object assumption
a new word refers to a whole object, not to actions, parts of objects, or properties of objects
New cards
26
The mutual-exclusivity assumption
children assume each object has only one name
New cards
27
The taxonomic assumption
- words refer to things of the same kind
New cards
28
shape bias
children preferentially attend to shape to determine category membership
New cards
29
Training the shape bias
17-month-olds were randomly assigned to 2 conditions
New cards
30
shape bias training

7 weeks of training (novel objects with the same shape, but different texture and color, were labeled with the same novel word)

  • control: no training

  • results: shape-bias training increased learning of object names but not other words

New cards
31
Challenges to word-learning assumptions
  • whole-object assumption must be overridden when a child learns names for parts or properties

  • mutual-exclusivity assumption must be overridden for synonyms and reference to objects at different taxonomic levels

  • taxonomic assumption must be overridden for proper names

New cards
32
Syntactic bootstrapping
children's ability to use syntactic cues to help them learn words
New cards
33
Syntax
the component of grammar that governs the order of words in sentences
New cards
34
Descriptive grammatical rules
implicit grammatical rules that all speakers of a language follow unconsciously
New cards
35
prescriptive grammatical rules
taught in classes and by parents, markers of etiquette and education
New cards
36
Adult syntax knowledge is a combination of...
linear order, grammatical categories, hierarchical structure
New cards
37
The productivity/generativity of language
we can produce an infinite number of sentences that we have never heard before
New cards
38
Vertical constructions
a sequence of one-word sentences that are meaningfully connected
New cards
39
Unanalyzed word combinations
sequences of words for which children have probably memorized the whole sequence rather than separate words
New cards
40
Open-class words
  • content words, mostly nouns, verbs and adjectives

  • open in the sense that we add to this class of words all the time

  • typical of two-word utterances

New cards
41
Closed-class words
  • fixed set of function words, including

  1. auxiliaries (can, do, will)

  2. prepositions (in, below, of)

  3. determiners (the, those, an)

  4. complementizers (that, which, who)

New cards
42
Early three-word speech
sounds a bit like a telegraph message, composed exclusively of open-class content words
New cards
43
Why don't children use more closed-class words?
  • because they are not necessary to convey meaning; they reserve their limited processing space for critical elements

  • they leave out elements that are phonetically less prominent

  • they may not understand the functions of closed-class words

New cards
44
Morphology
  • smallest unit that has meaning in a language

  • combinatorial

  • free ("truck" in trucks) or bound ("s" in trucks)

  • derivational (flawLESS, drinkABLE) or inflectional (walkED, walkING)

New cards
45
Morphological development in children acquiring English
  • takes children many months to reliably use even very common inflectional morphemes

  • different morphemes are acquired at different times but in a very similar order among children

New cards
46
Morphological development in children acquiring languages other than English
  • children acquiring rich systems of morphological inflections acquire grammatical morphemes as early/sometimes earlier than they acquire word order

  • following factors help children acquire morphemes earlier

  1. highly frequent use

  2. highly regular systems with few exceptions

  3. higher perceptual salience (ex. stressed syllables)

New cards
47
Expressing negation
children express negation early on before they can use adult-like syntax
New cards
48
Asking questions
  • yes/no questions: first questions are marked only by intonation

  • WH-questions: who, what when, where

  • later, children begin to produce auxiliaries and subject-auxiliary inversion

New cards
49
Individual differences in grammatical development
- some children start combining words into 2-word utterances before 18 months, while others do not do so until after 24 months
New cards
50
syntactic development is often measured
by the Mean Length of Utterance (MLU)
MLU can be measured in terms of words or morphemes
New cards
51
Nativist theory
  • the capacity for learning language is innate, including guidance about the abstract structure of all languages

  • learning a language is like learning to walk

New cards
52
Principles and parameters theory
- nativist
New cards
53
Universal Grammar is like a switchboard
a set of parameters, each with a few possible settings
- experience with a specific language "triggers" the correct parameter settings for that language
New cards
54
Constructivist theory
  • children construct their grammatical knowledge from their experiences with language

  • early grammatical knowledge may not be abstract children may be conservative in their early language production, not going beyond what they hear

New cards
55
Usage-based accounts
  • children acquire words and constructions which they combine in limited ways

  • learning is driven by children's communicative skills and ability to find patterns

New cards
56
The connectionist approach
  • connectionist nets make associations among elements in the input

  • what we see as "rules" emerge from the interaction of the input and the net

New cards
57
Different verbs have different argument structures
ditransitive, intransitive, and transitive
New cards
58
ditransitive
"give" has 3 arguments associated with it: giver, given, givee
New cards
59
transitive
"eat" has 2 arguments associated with it: eater, eaten -->
New cards
60
intransitive
"sleep" has 1 argument associated with it: sleeper -->
New cards
61
Evidence for item-specificity
  • diary study, age 16-24 months

  • child used limited sentence frames for most verbs

  • new utterances were most closely related to previous sentences with that specific verb, not that verb category

New cards
62
elicited production study
34-month-olds and 41-month-olds- heard a new verb in passive sentences
- only 20% of younger children and 55% of older children produced an active transitive sentence
New cards
63
Verb island hypothesis
  • each specific verb has its own set of rules

  • there is no general rule that applies to all verbs

New cards
64
Evidence for productivity
  • children commonly produce overregularizations and overgeneralizations

  • at first, children produce irregular forms correctly

  • then, they overregularize

  • later, they return to correct usage

New cards
65
Is grammatical knowledge abstract?
  • 2-year-olds children viewed videos on two screens

  • infants were familiarized with the stimuli using familiar verbs

New cards
66
test phase: animation
  • results: children were significantly above chance, consistent with abstract knowledge of verb argument structure

  • 21-month-olds were above chance at looking at the correct video when "bunny" and "duck" were changed to "girl" and "boy"

  • suggests that by 21 months, infants' knowledge of verb argument structure is abstract, not limited to specific verbs that they have learned

New cards
67
Evidence for abstractness
  • children produce rule-based forms they have never heard before

  • children comprehend new verbs based on their syntactic frames

New cards
68
The case for multiple systems (abstract and item-specific)
  • many researchers believe that children and adults sometimes use memorized forms and sometimes use abstract rules

  • regular pasts can be represented with rules (add -ed)

  • irregular forms must be memorized (go --> went, eat --> ate)

New cards
69
The tadpole-frog problem
If children's and adults' knowledge is qualitatively different, how does developmental change occur?
New cards
70
Solutions to tadpole-frog problem
- Solution 1: There are no tadpoles. There is developmental continuity because children have abstract, rule-based grammatical knowledge from the start.
New cards
71
Solution 2:
There are no frogs. There is developmental continuity because even adults' grammatical knowledge is not abstract.
New cards
72
Solution 3:
There are tadpoles and frogs. There is developmental discontinuity, accounted for by learning or maturation.
New cards
73
In the U.S., about _________% of people speak more than one language.
20-25
New cards
74
Simultaneous bilingualism
child learns multiple languages from birth
New cards
75
Sequential bilingualism
child begins to acquire another language after starting to acquire a first language
New cards
76
Majority language
spoken by the majority of individuals in a society
New cards
77
minority language
any language not spoken by the majority of the community
New cards
78
Heritage language
language of the culture in which a child was born
New cards
79
Language differentiation
child's knowledge that they are being exposed to two languages and eventual ability to separate them
New cards
80
Language differentiation hypotheses
fusion, autonomous differentiation, interdependent differentiation
New cards
81
Fusion
Initially, children do not differentiate languages, fusing them into one. Differentiation occurs later.
New cards
82
Autonomous differentiation
Children separate languages, acquiring each separately. Rate and process of development are independent in each language.
New cards
83
Interdependent differentiation:
Children separate languages, but they influence one other during acquisition.
New cards
84
Phonological differentiation
  • infants' perception becomes tuned to their native language from age 6-12 months

  • by 12 months, bilingual infants get better at distinguishing the phonemic contrasts in both languages

New cards
85
Do children create two different lexicons at the same time?
  • If children have a fused language, they should apply mutual exclusivity, and learn one label for each concept.

  • If children have differentiated languages, they can suspend mutual exclusivity to learn two labels for the same concept.

  • Result: Between 30-50% of bilingual children's vocabularies consist of translation equivalents.

New cards
86
Bilingual learners must have suspended mutual exclusivity assumption
suggests that their lexicons are differentiated
New cards
87
Syntactic differentiation
  • evidence suggests that bilingual children create separate syntactic systems

  • bilingual children, like bilingual adults, do mix words from both languages into the same sentences

New cards
88
Does bilingual language learning change the way language develops?
the overall course of development is very similar for monolingual and bilingual children
New cards
89
Does bilingual language learning change the rate of language development?
  • simultaneous bilingual infants and monolingual infants

  • observed at age 1 year 10 months, 2 years 1 month, and 2 years 6 months

  • at each age, parents reported for each language separately their child's vocabulary and syntax

  • results: bilingual learners lagged the monolingual learners in vocabulary in each language, and bilingual children were less likely than monolinguals to produce two-word utterances in each languag

New cards
90
Do bilinguals eventually catch up to monolinguals in a single language?
- vocabulary: possibly not, differences exist at all ages tested
New cards
91
- syntax and morphology:
yes by age 10, with consistent exposure and usage of both languages
New cards
92
Bilinguals' OVERALL language development is ______________________________ than monolinguals'.
the same as or better
New cards
93
Variable properties of bilingual environments
  • societal factors

  • heritage language culture

  • household composition

New cards
94
Does amount of exposure to language matter?
percentage of language exposure is correlated with vocabulary and syntax
New cards
95
Vocabulary in English is positively related to...
  • amount of exposure to English

  • number of different sources of English

  • amount of English that comes from native speakers

New cards
96
Recent work has found bilingual advantages for...
- theory of mind (TOM): understanding others' mental states - executive function: ability to control attention and to flexibly shift attention
New cards
97
Do bilingual children do better than monolinguals on language-specific TOM tasks, all TOM tasks, or all information-processing tasks?
- bilingual and monolingual children
New cards
98
- completed three tasks: Standard Theory of Mind, Modified Theory of Mind (language switch), control task (no mental states)
- results: bilinguals out-performed monolinguals on both theory of mind tasks (hypothesized to be due to general enhancement of executive function)
New cards
99
Do bilingual children have better executive function than monolinguals?
- bilingual 6-year-olds
New cards
100
color-shape task switching
- results: bilinguals were faster than monolinguals when switching between rules
New cards

Explore top notes

note Note
studied byStudied by 6 people
873 days ago
5.0(1)
note Note
studied byStudied by 17 people
884 days ago
5.0(1)
note Note
studied byStudied by 8 people
881 days ago
4.0(1)
note Note
studied byStudied by 32 people
905 days ago
5.0(1)
note Note
studied byStudied by 117 people
698 days ago
5.0(3)
note Note
studied byStudied by 9 people
432 days ago
4.5(2)
note Note
studied byStudied by 6 people
761 days ago
5.0(1)
note Note
studied byStudied by 23112 people
699 days ago
4.7(77)

Explore top flashcards

flashcards Flashcard (32)
studied byStudied by 22 people
418 days ago
5.0(1)
flashcards Flashcard (26)
studied byStudied by 46 people
408 days ago
5.0(1)
flashcards Flashcard (287)
studied byStudied by 6 people
37 days ago
5.0(1)
flashcards Flashcard (177)
studied byStudied by 4 people
878 days ago
5.0(1)
flashcards Flashcard (36)
studied byStudied by 18 people
152 days ago
5.0(1)
flashcards Flashcard (288)
studied byStudied by 18 people
828 days ago
5.0(1)
flashcards Flashcard (141)
studied byStudied by 15 people
382 days ago
5.0(1)
flashcards Flashcard (400)
studied byStudied by 5 people
289 days ago
5.0(1)
robot