1/51
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
Phonology
System of sounds (Phonemes, e.g. ‘b’ vs ‘d’)
Semantics
Meaning of words and combinations
Grammar
Structure of language (arranging words into sentences)
Pragmatics
Social rules for language (context use, turn taking etc.).
Effective social communication.
Language development requires:
Comprehension
Production
Learning/Empiricist perspective of language development
Skinner (1957) - children are reinforced with grammatically correct speech.
Adults shape children’s speech by selectively reinforcing babbling that sounds like words.
Once words have been shaped, reinforcement is withheld until child begins to combine words.
Learning perspective of language development
Bandura (1971) - children listen and imitate language of older companions, caregivers teach language by modelling language
Nativist perspective of language development
Humans are biologically programmed to acquire language.
Chomsky - language acquisition device (LAD)
Slobin (1985) - language-making capacity (LMC)
Sensitive periods
Chomsky’s language acquisition device (LAD)
An inborn linguistic processor that is activated by verbal input, contains a universal grammar
Slobin’s (1985) language-making capacity (LMC)
Children have this inborn LMC, which is a set of cognitive/perceptual abilities highly specialised for language learning
Interactionist perspective of language development
Results from complex interplay of biological maturation, cognitive development and linguistic environment.
Suggests children worldwide develop language at similar pace because members of same species.
Children biologically prepared to learn language not because of LAD/LMC but because they have powerful brain that matures slowly - language develops as children communicate with people.
Factors affecting language development
Damage to left hemisphere
Sensitive periods
Experience (child-directed speech, non-verbal games)
Damage to left hemisphere leads to…
Aphasisa - a communication disorder that makes it difficult to speak, understand language, read, and write
Broca’s area affects…
Speech production
Wernicke’s area affects…
Speech comprehension
Sensitive periods
Times in development when the brain is particularly receptive to learning language skills.
E.g. case of Genie who was brought up with dogs and never learned language.
Child-directed speech
Close proximity, exaggerated facial expressions, repetition, higher pitched voice.
Natural in most cultures.
Parents gradually increase length and complexity of child-directed speech as children get older
Playing non-verbal games
Teaches turn-taking, e.g. pat-a-cake, peek-a-boo
Prelinguistic phase
From birth to 6-months, although infants start to process sounds in the womb (DeCasper & Spence, 1986)
Cat in the womb study
Showed infants preferred to listen to intonation of the passages they had been read prenatally.
1-2mth infants discriminate between different phonemes.
Discrimination includes speech sounds across different languages.
This decreases at 6-12 months (Werker & Tees, 2005)
Preparation for speech production
Different patterns of crying (hunger, anger, pain), increasingly social in nature.
Cooing from 1 month
Cooing
Making repetitive vowel sounds signalling pleasure, varying in volume and pitch
Proto-conversations
Bateson (1975) - mothers tend to vocalise when their infants have finished vocalising, which stimulates turn-taking.
These interactions evolve into triadic interactions when infants point to objects:
Proto-imperative - requests for objects/actions
Proto-declarative - comments on objects/actions
Development of speech production
1 month - cooing and laughing
4-6 months - babbling and vocal play
6-10 months - canonical babbling
Canonical/reduplicated babbling
Babbling repetitive vowel-consonant combinations.
Gradually develop intonation
Shift to sounds that are heard most (combinations of sounds that sound like words)
Receptive language
Ability to understand and comprehend spoken language.
Evidence of receptive language before expressive language.
Fenson (1994)
Examined mother's’ report of number of words understood by infants.
10 months = 30 words
13 months = 100 words
Bergelson & Swingley (2012)
Infants begin associating highly familiar words with highly familiar referents surprisingly early.
At 6-9 months, infants knew meaning of many common nouns
Most parents did not realise this
How do infants learn meaning of words
Learning in ‘constraint’ - (e.g. whole object constraint, mutual exclusivity constraint)
Children construct semantic system of words relating to one another
Facilitated by their acquisition of semantic relations
Learn that objects can be referred to by more than one word and how words relate to one another (e.g. opposites)
Helps children identify gaps in vocab
Production of first words
10-15 months, can be of any sound but they are used consistently in presence of object/situation.
They condense meaning (e.g. “want milk”).
Holophrase period
12-18 months, using a single word for a whole phrase
Overextension
One word applied in a broader context than is appropriate
Vocabulary spurt
16-24 months
Nelson (1973)
First words tend to be nominals (ball, dog), then specific nominals (mummy) and action words (give, bye bye)
Facilitators of vocab spurt
Parents:
Label, stress, repeat new words
Play naming games
Introducing new words depending on context
Spatial consistency
Children:
Fast mapping
Using pragmatic cues
Inferring meaning
Cross-situational word learning
Spatial consistency
Infants learn label of objects more readily when object is in same location each time they are labelled
Fast mapping
Process of rapidly learning new word from context, e.g. hearing the contrastive use of a familiar and unfamiliar word
Pragmatic cues
Eye gaze, labelling
Inferring meaning
Taking cues from linguistic context and syntactic bootstrapping (children learn word meanings by using the grammatical structure of sentences)
Cross-situational word learning
Repeated correspondence between words heard and objects observed
Brown (1973)
First sentences combining two words came at around 18-24 months.
Telegraphic speech - short and simple, grammatical markers missing, may reflect multiple meanings depending on context
Fenson et al. (1994)
Very strong correlation between vocab size and complexity of child’s sentences
Grammar development at 2-3 years
Telegraphic speech ceases
Add inflections (e.g. ‘ing’ added to verbs)
Beginning to form questions and negatives
Grammar development at 3-4
Overregularisation.
Apply very basic rules to irregular words (e.g. ‘ed’ applied to all verbs to form past tense)
But form more complex sentences using conjunctions (‘and’, ‘but’) to connect two or more ideas, embedded clauses
When is there major strides in grammar
Between 1-4 years
Hart & Risley (1995)
Differences in children’s vocab size and amount parents talked to their children was dependent on social class - much lower for working class
DeLoache et al. (2010)
Infants who learn from parents performed best.
Infants in video-learning condition did not perform better than in control condition.
Newport and Johnson (1989)
The ability to learn languages declines after children reach the age of approximately
Before the age of 7, children can learn a second language with similar proficiency to native speakers.
"Whole object constraint" refers to the idea that
Children expect that a word will always refer to a whole object
What is “mutual exclusivity” in word learning?
Children assume a new word refers to an unfamiliar object
Telegraphic speech
Simple two-word phrases missing grammatical markers
Werker & Tees (2005) found that children's ability to discriminate between different language sounds decreased at _____ months because of neural commitment. This is a characteristic of the prelinguistic phase.
6-12 months