Love + Prosocial 1 - Oxytocin & Vasopressin (vs. testosterone) - Differential Psychology

0.0(0)
learnLearn
examPractice Test
spaced repetitionSpaced Repetition
heart puzzleMatch
flashcardsFlashcards
Card Sorting

1/44

flashcard set

Earn XP

Description and Tags

Differential Psychology - Differential & Clinical Psychology

Psychology

Study Analytics
Name
Mastery
Learn
Test
Matching
Spaced

No study sessions yet.

45 Terms

1
New cards

Prosocial effects of oxytocin and vasopressin (4 points)

  • Maternal and paternal behaviour

  • Attachment in young

  • Social recognition

  • Sexual behaviour

  • Pair bonding and social bonding (i.e. adult affiliation)

  • Empathy and Theory of mind (humans)

  • Interpersonal warmth & Trust (humans)

  • Reduces anxiety (cautious social distance?)

2
New cards

Walum et al. (2008) - AVPR1a gene (3 points)

  • If both your copies of this gene are a particular variant allele, your marriage is of poorer quality.

  • Looked at both animal models and manipulation of genes, IVs, etc, to understand indiv diff.

  • AVPR1a gene: Produces vasopressin receptors.

3
New cards

Bartels & Zeki (2004) - maternal vs. romantic love (5 points)

  • fMRI studies in humans, UCL. People who had 'recently fallen in love.’

  • Looked at differences in romantic love and maternal love.

  • Neuroimaging subtraction technique:

    • Task activation pattern MINUS control pattern = ‘Core’ activation specifically relating to task.

    • Should be left with brain regions focused on romantic love.

  • “Both types of attachment (ie maternal and romantic love) activated regions specific to each, as well as overlapping regions in the brain's reward system that coincide with areas rich in oxytocin and vasopressin receptors.”

4
New cards

Insel & Young (2001) - prairie vole (3 points)

  • A male and female prairie vole typically form a life-long pair bond after one mating/cohabitation episode - rare for animals.

  • Good for testing neurobiological basis of romantic love.

  • Links between pair bonding and biparental care? Yes, but not always.

5
New cards

Why does biparental care necessary for some species?

Male and female team up as economic unit? - necessary need for many resources.

6
New cards

Lim et al. (2004) - prairie vs. meadow vole

“Prairie voles (microtus ochrogaster) exhibit a monogamous social structure in nature, whereas closely related meadow voles (microtus pennysylvanicus) are solitary and polygamous.”

7
New cards

Monogamous voles

Praire mole

8
New cards

Polyamorous voles

  • Meadow voles

  • Montane vole

9
New cards

What percentage of mammalian species have a monogamous social structure and what does this indicate? (3 points)

< 5%. Indicates:

  • Lasting pair bond between adult mates

  • Both male and female care for offspring

10
New cards

Is oxytocin or vasopressin more important in females?

Females

11
New cards

Is oxytocin or vasopressin more important in males?

Vasopressin

12
New cards

Which forebrain regions do oxytocin and vasopressin act in? (2 points)

  • Ventral pallidum

  • Caudate

  • Putamen

  • Nucleus accumbens

13
New cards

Ross & Young (2009) - oxytocin

Oxytocin promotes adult pair bonding: oxytocin receptor density is increased in monogamous vole.

14
New cards

Ross & Young (2009) - oxytocin (prairie vole vs. mouse vs. rat) (3 points)

  • Compare prairie vole to other animals - shows Oxytocin receptor density in different parts of the brain.

  • Oxytocin receptor density in nucleus accumbens (Nacc) and caudate and putamen and prefrontal cortex is clearly very much higher (black staining) in the prairie vole (monogamous) compared to the rat and mouse (which are NOT monogamous).

  • Reasonable hypothesis: more oxytocin receptors may predispose to increased pair bonding?

<ul><li><p>Compare prairie vole to other animals - shows Oxytocin receptor density in different parts of the brain. </p></li><li><p>Oxytocin receptor density in nucleus accumbens (Nacc) and caudate and putamen and prefrontal cortex is clearly very much higher (black staining) in the prairie vole (monogamous) compared to the rat and mouse (which are NOT monogamous). </p></li><li><p>Reasonable hypothesis: more oxytocin receptors may predispose to increased pair bonding?</p></li></ul><p></p>
15
New cards

Ross & Young (2009) - oxytocin (prairie vole vs. montane vole) (3 points)

  • Better is a within-animal comparison.

  • Oxytocin receptor density is clearly much higher in the prairie vole, the monogamous species, than the non-monogamous montane vole.

  • Thus more oxytocin receptors may predispose to pair bonding?

<ul><li><p>Better is a within-animal comparison.</p></li><li><p>Oxytocin receptor density is clearly much higher in the prairie vole, the monogamous species, than the non-monogamous montane vole. </p></li><li><p>Thus more oxytocin receptors may predispose to pair bonding?</p></li></ul><p></p>
16
New cards

Partner preference test (voles) (2 points)

  • Cohabitation period - Bring together a male and female vole together for a fixed cohabitation period (c. 18-24 hours.) Observe effect on behaviour. They may mate (check for mating.) Subject may be male or female. Partner and stranger = opposite sex. Typically female in oxytocin studies, male in vasopressin studies.

  • Test period - Then observe interactions over a test period (e.g. 3 hours). Ask eg. How much time do these sexual partners spend side-by-side? Then compare to ‘stranger’ vole who has not mated with subject.

17
New cards

Ross & Young (2009) - Partner Preference Test (5 points)

  • Oxytocin receptor ANTAGONIST infused into 3 different oxytocin-receptor-rich regions.

  • OTA - Oxytocin antagonist - stops Oxytocin from binding.

  • Reversal of behaviour - monogamous behaviour no longer exhibited.

  • Infusion of the OXT-R antagonist into the nucleus accumbens OR prefrontal cortex was sufficient to BLOCK preference for the mating partner over a stranger.

  • Conclusion: Oxytocin receptors in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex are required for normal adult pair bonding.

<ul><li><p>Oxytocin receptor ANTAGONIST infused into 3 different oxytocin-receptor-rich regions.</p></li><li><p>OTA - Oxytocin antagonist - stops Oxytocin from binding. </p></li><li><p>Reversal of behaviour - monogamous behaviour no longer exhibited. </p></li><li><p>Infusion of the OXT-R antagonist into the nucleus accumbens OR prefrontal cortex was sufficient to BLOCK preference for the mating partner over a stranger. </p></li><li><p>Conclusion: Oxytocin receptors in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex are required for normal adult pair bonding.</p></li></ul><p></p>
18
New cards

Oxytocin is released during… (3 points)

  • Prairie vole mating

  • In rats & sheep during vaginocervical stimulation

  • In human sexual behaviour (not much studied)

19
New cards

In relationship to mating behaviour, in females, there is some evidence for a positive relationship between…

Subjectivity intensity of orgasm & oxytocin release

20
New cards

Murphy et al. (1987) - oxytocin and vasopressin peaks

Both signals show strong increases at arousal/ejaculation.

<p>Both signals show strong increases at arousal/ejaculation.</p>
21
New cards

Vasopressin in mating behaviour

Helps in seeking what an animal would like in a sexual partner.

22
New cards

Oxytocin in mating behaviour

Responsible for pleasurable feeling during sex (in females).

23
New cards

Lim et al. (2004) - vasopressin (prairie vs. meadow voles) - study (4 points)

  • Changing the vasopressin V1a gene’s expression made the solitary meadow vole show much increased partner preference.

  • Red text: Comparison of behaviour in prairie and meadow voles. A, B) Although prairie voles and meadow voles are similar in physical appearance, prairie voles are highly affiliative as depicted here in ‘huddling’ side by side (a), whereas meadow voles are solitary (b).

  • C, D) Partner preference test. After mating and cohabitating with a female, a male prairie vole tended to spend significantly more time in contact with the partner (filled columns) than the stranger (open columns) (P , 0.05, Student’s t-test) (c), whereas meadow voles do not form partner preferences and spent relatively little time huddling with either female (d).

  • Change in behaviour - meadow vole behaves much more like prairie vole - monogamous behaviour. Effect is specific to partner, not stranger.

<ul><li><p>Changing the vasopressin V1a gene’s expression made the solitary meadow vole show much increased partner preference.</p></li><li><p>Red text: Comparison of behaviour in prairie and meadow voles. A, B) Although prairie voles and meadow voles are similar in physical appearance, prairie voles are highly affiliative as depicted here in ‘huddling’ side by side (a), whereas meadow voles are solitary (b). </p></li><li><p>C, D) Partner preference test. After mating and cohabitating with a female, a male prairie vole tended to spend significantly more time in contact with the partner (filled columns) than the stranger (open columns) (P , 0.05, Student’s t-test) (c), whereas meadow voles do not form partner preferences and spent relatively little time huddling with either female (d).</p></li><li><p>Change in behaviour - meadow vole behaves much more like prairie vole - monogamous behaviour. Effect is specific to partner, not stranger.</p></li></ul><p></p>
24
New cards

Lim et al. (2004) - vasopressin (prairie vs. meadow voles) - summary of findings (4 points)

  • Larry Young’s group artificially over-expressed the V1aR gene (DNA) in a reward region of the brain called the ventral pallium in male meadow voles (NOT monogamous, & they do not show partner preference as measured by huddling).

  • V1aR gene over-expression increased the number of Vasopressin 1A receptors (proteins) on the post-synaptic membranes of vasopressin-responsive neurons (in that brain region). The same amount of vasopressin released would now have a much larger effect on the vasopressin-responsive neurons.

  • Male meadow voles who received the V1aR Ventral Pallium over-expression showed unusually (non species-typical) strong huddling!

  • Conclusion: Higher V1A receptor density increases adult pair bonding.

<ul><li><p>Larry Young’s group artificially over-expressed the V1aR gene (DNA) in a reward region of the brain called the ventral pallium in male meadow voles (NOT monogamous, &amp; they do not show partner preference as measured by huddling). </p></li><li><p>V1aR gene over-expression increased the number of Vasopressin 1A receptors (proteins) on the post-synaptic membranes of vasopressin-responsive neurons (in that brain region). The same amount of vasopressin released would now have a much larger effect on the vasopressin-responsive neurons. </p></li><li><p>Male meadow voles who received the V1aR Ventral Pallium over-expression showed unusually (non species-typical) strong huddling! </p></li><li><p><strong>Conclusion</strong>: Higher V1A receptor density increases adult pair bonding.</p></li></ul><p></p>
25
New cards

In pair bonding, blocking oxytocin and vasopressin receptors eliminates…

Post-coital huddling

26
New cards

Vole studies examine… (2 points)

  • Species differences (e.g. vole vs mouse, polygamous vs monogamous vole)

  • Experimentally-induced differences (exogenous differences) (over-expressing or blocking neuropeptide receptors).

27
New cards

Walum et al. (2008) - AVPR1A gene (2 points)

  • Genetic variations in this vasopressin is associated with pair-bonding behaviour in humans.

  • This is the same gene that Larry Young’s group overexpressed in meadow vole males and made meadow voles more ‘monogamous’.

  • 'Worse' (risk) gene - if you have both copies of risk allele, more likely to score worse on indexes of romantic behaviours.

  • Women in relationships with men who have 1 or 2 ‘334’ alleles say that their relationship is of lower quality than those women with men who have ZERO ‘334’ alleles.

<ul><li><p>Genetic variations in this vasopressin is associated with pair-bonding behaviour in humans.</p></li><li><p>This is the same gene that Larry Young’s group overexpressed in meadow vole males and made meadow voles more ‘monogamous’.</p></li><li><p>'Worse' (risk) gene - if you have both copies of risk allele, more likely to score worse on indexes of romantic behaviours.</p></li><li><p>Women in relationships with men who have 1 or 2 ‘334’ alleles say that their relationship is of lower quality than those women with men who have ZERO ‘334’ alleles.</p></li></ul><p></p>
28
New cards

‘Partner Bonding scale’ questionnaire (3 points)

  • Consisting of 13 items overall, drawn from:

    • Dyadic adjustment scale (DAS) (7 items)

    • Support Seeking and Giving (SSG) assessment (5 items)

    • Marital instability scale (MIS) (1 item)

29
New cards

Serious Marital Threat question

Has subject experienced marital crisis or threat of divorce during the last year? (from a Life Events questionnaire).

30
New cards

According to Walum et al. (2008), men with 1 or 2 copies of the alleles in the AVPR1A gene… (2 points)

  • You are less likely to get married (an objective fact)

  • You are more likely to experience a serious threat to your relationship (your self-report)

  • You show a less-bonded pattern of relationship with your partner (your self-report)

  • Your female partner reports less quality in the relationship, particularly regarding affection

  • Finally, no significant effects for female polymorphisms of the AVPR1a gene.

31
New cards

Walum et al. (2008) - interpretations (4 points)

  • Differences in romantic partnership behaviour may owe in part to genes relating to vasopressin (this is just re one allele on one gene).

  • Animal models’ usefulness: Striking conservation of gene function, promotes pair bonding, particularly the male of the pair.

  • While the effect sizes seem relatively modest bear in mind:

    • This was a study of couples together for at least 5 years. Perhaps many men with 334 allele aren’t typically in a relationship, or only in short ones. Thus the present study may underestimate the way in which the 334 allele predisposes against ‘marriage’.

    • ‘Marriage’ is rather complex. E.g. consider influence of Drive, Duty, conscientiousness, and religious and cultural beliefs.

32
New cards

Ditzen et al. (2009) - oxytocin in prosocial behaviour (2 points)

  • Ps given oxytocin showed more positive than negative behaviour.

  • Can vary levels of prosocial behaviour in men and women by administering oxytocin.

<ul><li><p>Ps given oxytocin showed more positive than negative behaviour.</p></li><li><p>Can vary levels of prosocial behaviour in men and women by administering oxytocin.</p></li></ul><p></p>
33
New cards

Ditzen et al. (2009) - oxytocin and cortisol (3 points)

  • Subjects given oxytocin show lower cortisol levels after couple conflict, particularly in women.

  • Oxytocin reduces stress

  • Exogenous variation

<ul><li><p>Subjects given oxytocin show lower cortisol levels after couple conflict, particularly in women. </p></li><li><p>Oxytocin reduces stress </p></li><li><p>Exogenous variation</p></li></ul><p></p>
34
New cards

Gonzaga et al. (2006) - oxytocin, romantic love, sexual desire - procedure (4 points)

  • Subjects: 25 young women recount and relive a positive emotion regarding love or infatuation with their male partners present (typically 2 minutes long).

  • Blood drawn from subjects during this and earlier baseline condition, later tested for oxytocin content (& other conditions not relevant here).

  • Scorers observe non-verbal behaviour in women:

    • Affiliative (romantic), e.g. affirmative head nods, Duchenne smiles, leaning towards partner.

    • Sexual, e.g. lip licking, lip puckering, tongue protrusions.

35
New cards

Gonzaga et al. (2006) - oxytocin, romantic love, sexual desire - findings and conclusions (5 points)

  • Romantic display strongly correlates with Oxytocin release.

  • To be precise: Duration of romantic displays as % of duration of romantic episode narration (~ 2 minutes) correlated positively with increase in oxytocin relative to first baseline.

  • No correlation with overt sexual displays.

  • Summary: Oxytocin release may support romantic behaviour.

  • Causality unclear: virtuous cycle? Oxytocin-behaviour-oxytocin?

36
New cards

Strategy - oxytocin and vasopressin in love and desire (4 points)

  • Co-operative: brings the benefits of monogamy, promotes ‘love’.

  • ‘Quality of bond not quantity’

  • Commitment to existing bonds, i.e. enhancement of adult pair bond and parenting.

  • Reduced aggression with mate & progeny

37
New cards

Strategy - testosterone in love and desire

  • Competitive: brings the benefits of polygamy (infidelity), promotes ‘sex’.

  • ‘Quantity of mates not quality’

  • Focus upon more partners

  • More attractive partner? i.e. more ‘mating’, less ‘pair bonding’, more children

  • Higher aggression with mate & progeny?

38
New cards

Speculation of strategies (oxytocin, vasopressin, testosterone) (4 points)

  • Pure strategy of either not that evolutionarily stable?

  • If everyone is ‘high testosterone’ only, children die at too high rates, or do not thrive, from abandonment & low parental investment. Influx of ‘high oxytocin/vasopressin’ will spread because children suddenly thrive to adulthood, and then produce children themselves.

  • If everyone is ‘high oxytocin/vasopressin’ only, network is highly vulnerable to influx of ‘high testosterone’ strategy. A bit like loafers.

  • So humans exhibit a mixture of both strategies, and individual differences.

39
New cards

Burnham et al. (2003); Gray et al. (2002) - testosterone (5 points)

  • Testosterone levels are lower in adult pair bonding

  • Is the effect due to predisposition or experience? One or both?

    • Predisposition?: High Testosterone man chooses not to get married and have kids?

    • Experience?: Romantic and paternal activity actively lowers testosterone?

  • Looks like both factors play a role but have a look at the literature.

  • Difference in married, not married, and no children.

<ul><li><p>Testosterone levels are lower in adult pair bonding </p></li><li><p>Is the effect due to predisposition or experience? One or both? </p><ul><li><p><strong>Predisposition?</strong>: High Testosterone man chooses not to get married and have kids? </p></li><li><p><strong>Experience?</strong>: Romantic and paternal activity actively lowers testosterone? </p></li></ul></li><li><p>Looks like both factors play a role but have a look at the literature.</p></li><li><p>Difference in married, not married, and no children.</p></li></ul><p></p>
40
New cards

Booth & Dabbs - testosterone (4 points)

  • Variation in testosterone signals predicts individual differences in pair bonding.

  • Hi-test men > doubly likely to not get married; doubly likely to divorce; double likely to be unfaithful.

  • What is good about study:

    • Adjustment for age

    • Many different facets of this form of adult pair bonding (marriage)

<ul><li><p>Variation in testosterone signals predicts individual differences in pair bonding.</p></li><li><p>Hi-test men &gt; doubly likely to not get married; doubly likely to divorce; double likely to be unfaithful.</p></li><li><p>What is good about study: </p><ul><li><p>Adjustment for age </p></li><li><p>Many different facets of this form of adult pair bonding (marriage)</p></li></ul></li></ul><p></p>
41
New cards

Mascaro, Hackett & Rilling - parenting and mating (4 points)

  • The biology of human males represents a trade-off between:

    • effort spent on mating

    • effort spent on parenting

  • A simple interpretation and hypothesis:

    • The effort on mating is mediated by testosterone

    • The effort on parenting is mediated by oxytocin

42
New cards

Chin et al. (2020) - testosterone (5 points)

  • Looked at WLW relationships.

  • Birth mothers reported HIGHER relationship satisfaction when their partners had LOWER testosterone (TEST).

  • Non-birth mothers reported higher perceived support and relationship commitment when their partners had LOWER testosterone.

  • Non-birth mothers with LOWER testosterone reported receiving MORE support, and HIGHER relationship satisfaction, commitment & investment.

  • Higher scores indicate better satisfaction, investment etc, so negative correlations mean HIGHER testosterone levels were associated with LOWER relationship quality between these couples.

<ul><li><p>Looked at WLW relationships.</p></li><li><p>Birth mothers reported HIGHER relationship satisfaction when their partners had LOWER testosterone (TEST). </p></li><li><p>Non-birth mothers reported higher perceived support and relationship commitment when their partners had LOWER testosterone. </p></li><li><p>Non-birth mothers with LOWER testosterone reported receiving MORE support, and HIGHER relationship satisfaction, commitment &amp; investment.</p></li><li><p>Higher scores indicate better satisfaction, investment etc, so negative correlations mean HIGHER testosterone levels were associated with LOWER relationship quality between these couples.</p></li></ul><p></p>
43
New cards

Harris, Vernon & Boomsma (1998) - testosterone (3 points)

  • Demonstrated that testosterone is heritable.

  • Correlations in MZ twins > Correlations in DZ twins

  • Identical twin values (MZ) are very similar (in both males & females) and higher than for non-identical twins.

<ul><li><p>Demonstrated that testosterone is heritable.</p></li><li><p>Correlations in MZ twins &gt; Correlations in DZ twins </p></li><li><p>Identical twin values (MZ) are very similar (in both males &amp; females) and higher than for non-identical twins.</p></li></ul><p></p>
44
New cards

Heim et al. (2009) - childhood abuse (3 points)

  • Childhood abuse/neglect lowers adult oxytocin.

  • Two possible interpretations, not mutually exclusive

    • Low-loving interactions (hugs/cuddles/praise) gradually reduce Oxytocin

    • Protective mechanism – ‘shutting down’ normal levels of prosocial responsiveness because these would lead to more stress/pain

<ul><li><p>Childhood abuse/neglect lowers adult oxytocin.</p></li><li><p>Two possible interpretations, not mutually exclusive </p><ul><li><p>Low-loving interactions (hugs/cuddles/praise) gradually reduce Oxytocin </p></li><li><p>Protective mechanism – ‘shutting down’ normal levels of prosocial responsiveness because these would lead to more stress/pain</p></li></ul></li></ul><p></p>
45
New cards

McQuaid et al. (2013) - childhood maltreatment, depression (3 points)

  • Might seek positive reinforcement more with this allele - causes more negative treatment.

  • The GG/GA alleles of the rs53576 SNP of the Oxytocin Receptor typically confer increased empathy & trust; but under high levels of childhood maltreatment, this prosocial predisposition becomes maladaptive.

    • Overly loving, trusting, giving to e.g. callous, psychopathic parents is not good for you