1/68
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
child-directed speech
modified adult speech to a child
adult-directed speech
adult speech directed to other older children or adults
infant-directed singing
used cross-culturally and share many characteristics to motherese/parentese
- mothers moderate their voice in different ways when engaging in this with infants (higher pitch, greater emotional expressions, sustained vowels, slow tempo, gliding between pitch levels)
rhymes
patterned alliterations, rhythms, and repetitions of words in combination with sequenced actions of the parent and child provide a social structure for engaging in the routine
storytelling
structured, narrative-style speech that is typically very simple in terms of structure (predictable or repetitive phrases throughout)
contingent responding
reciprocal interaction between caregiver and infant
joint reference/joint attention
allow both to engage with each other on a specific topic
stage 1 of joint reference
4-6 weeks: caregiver places object where child can see it and calls attention to the object "look!"
6 months: child may recognize caregiver's pitch pattern as a signal to establish joint attention
stage 2 of joint reference
7 months: child begins to demonstrate efforts to communicate intentionally (pointing)
8 months: child may reach for an object and look at caregiver for a response
stage 3 of joint reference
8-12 months: child uses combination of gestures and vocalizations to indicate interests in objects
stage 4 of joint reference
12 months: child exercises control over topic; produces names of objects and events
characteristics of child-directed speech
acoustic: higher pitcher levels, loudness range
timing: more regular rhythm, slower speaking, pausing increased
syntactic: simpler syntactic constructions, repetition
other: exaggerated facial expressions, references to objects
expansion
using the utterance in a more complex adult-like phrase or sentence
- child: "mommy work"
- caregiver: "mommy went to work"
extension
extending past simple expansion to include additional semantic information
- child: "mommy go"
- caregiver: "mommy went to work"
importance of child-directed speech in early development
- impact a child's language and cognitive development
- impact academic success
- children with disabilities (autism): very important!
auditory processing of speech: development
1st year: processing of language (sounds, syllables, etc)
2nd year: early vocab development (50 words) and sorting out meanings
3rd year: child will have vocab of 500 words
fast mapping
act of learning a new concept after limited exposure, commonly used by young children when acquiring language
slow mapping
gradual process over time of expanding, reorganizing, and fine-tuning word meanings as words are acquired and the relationship between words develops along with cognition and linguistic attainments
acquisition of words divided into 3 hypotheses
1. semantic feature
2. functional core
3. prototype
semantic feature hypothesis
idea that the order of appearance of words in the course of a child's language acquisition is governed by the type and complexity of the semantic features they contain
(ex: dog = furry, 4 legs, etc.)
functional core hypothesis
idea that children do not categorize things according to perceptual attributes (e.g., shapes and size), but rather develop meanings of early words based on their actions if they are animate objects, or on the objects performed on them if they are inanimate objects; children form concepts by viewing objects in terms of the whole, not a sum of parts or features
(ex: dog = bark)
prototype hypothesis
idea that children rely on both perceptual features and functions of words to develop early meanings; children build mental models of a conceptual category—an ideal or representative example (prototype) that other members of this group must resemble
(ex: new experience compared to old)
reflexive crying
a newborn cries automatically in response to discomfort, such as being cold or hungry
cooing
a baby produces early vowel-like sounds, such as "ahh" and "ooo"
echolalia
imitation or repetition of words, phrases, or whole sentences spoken by another person
transitional/marginal babbling
consists of single consonants and vowels that are made in a row, such as "a, a, a," and "da, da, da"
reduplicated babbling
more specific term for babbling involving repeated consonant/vowel (CV) syllables (e.g., "mamama")
variegated babbling
changes in consonant/vowel combinations "dadu"
jargon babbling
intonational changes added to syllable productions to give impression of sentence-like behavior
protowords (vocables)
a baby consistently uses a made-up sound, such as "gaga", to refer to their favorite stuffed animal
(productions unique to each child)
development of speech acts: perlocutionary
refers to the actual effect that the utterance has on the listener
development of speech acts: illocutionary
refers to the speaker's intended meaning or communicative force behind the utterance
development of speech acts: locutionary
refers to the literal meaning of an utterance
overextension
child refers "doggy" to any four legged animal with fur (too broad of a category)
underextension
child refers "book" to Peter Rabbit, then seeing Three Blind Mice book, refers "mice" to that item (instead of book)
deictic gestures
use of pointing, showing, or reaching for something to call attention to or indicate an object or event
representational gestures
signify some features of an object or its function; have meaning independent of the objects (e.g., holding a fist to the ear to mean "telephone")
Halliday's expanded functional list (1975)
1. interacting = to maintain contact with others
2. regulatory = to control the behavior of others
3. personal = to express emotion or interest
4. heuristic = to explore and categorize
5. instrumental = to satisfy needs or wants
6. imagine = to accompany or support a play activity
7. informative = to share knowledge with others
prodeclarative
primitive speech act used to establish social interaction and direct a caregiver's attention; use of a gesture (sometimes paired with a vocalization) to gain attention to an object or event
protoimperative
primitive speech act used as a request for objects or actions as well as to control and manipulate the behaviors of others
Dore's primitive speech acts (1975)
1. labeling = to identify an object
2. answering = to respond to caregiver's inquiry
3. requesting action = to elict an actionable response from caregiver
4. requesting answer = to elict a communicative response from caregiver
5. calling = to gain attention
6. greeting = to acknowledge
7. protesting = to reject an object or action
8. repeating/imitating = to reproduce at least part of what caregiver said
9. practicing = to produce verbal patterns without anyone in environment
substantive words
words that refer to objects or events that have perceptual or functional features in common (e.g., doggie, horsie, truck)
agents
words that refer to things that cause action
objects
words that refer to things receiving action
relational words
words that identify relationships among people, objects, and events; these words reflect a child's understanding of space, time, amount, dimensions, and kinship
presupposition
assumption that speakers make concerning what their listener knows about the subject of their conversation
Grammatical categories of word type (Nelson, 1973)
1. nominals (specific and general)
2. action words
3. modifiers
4. personal-social words
5. functional words
presupposition
an assumption the speaker makes concerning what the listener knows about the subject of the conversation
stage 1 of syntactic development
12-26 months: semantic roles and grammatical relations
stage 2 of syntactic development
27-30 months: grammatical morphemes and the modulation of meanings
stage 3 of syntactic development
31-34 months: modalities of the sample sentence
stage 4 of syntactic development
35-40 months: embedding of one sentence within another
stage 5 of syntactic development
41-46 months: coordination of simple sentences and propositional sentences
morpheme
in a language, the smallest unit that carries meaning; may be a word or a part of a word (such as a prefix)
unbound/free morpheme
"root" of word (the "to pack" verb in "unpacks")
bound morpheme
morphemes that are attached to words and cannot stand alone (e.g., possessive form of -s)
derivational morpheme
morphemes added to the beginning (e.g., re-, un-) and end (e.g., -ly, -un) of words; prefixes and suffixes
(the "to undo" in "unpacks")
inflectional morpheme
suffixes that change the meaning of words by marking grammatical adjustments for things such as plurality, possession, and verb tense
(the 3rd-person present in "unpacks")
mean length of utterance (MLU)
a calculation of the average number of morphemes a child produces in a representative sample of utterances
310 morphemes/100 utterances = 3.1 MLU
early stage 1 MLU
using one-word utterances, MLU of 1-1.5, age range between 12 and 22 months
late stage 1 MLU
using two-word utterances, MLU of 1.5-2, age range between 22 and 26 months
pivot-open grammar
proposed primitive form of grammar which suggests that children have two classes of words: open and pivot; concept used to explain the structure behind two-word phrases often used by children
pivot words
words that cannot be used alone or combined with other pivot words but can be used in conjunction with almost any other word a child has learned, and are used in either the first or second position of two-word utterances
open words
words that can be used alone and in combination with other open words, in combination with pivot words, and in either the first or second position of two-word utterances
semantic-syntactic rules
underlying rules for combining words and grammar to produce meaningful utterances; concept that emphasizes that meaning precedes and influences form
semantic relations
agent + action ("doggy sit")
pragmatics
component of language regarding the ability to functionally use language in social contexts; the rules for social language needed to establish and maintain relationships with others
(the functions or intention: getting things done)
mathetic pragmatics
related to science or learning
informative pragmatics
giving new information to others