Social Perception + Attribution (Week 3)

0.0(0)
learnLearn
examPractice Test
spaced repetitionSpaced Repetition
heart puzzleMatch
flashcardsFlashcards
Card Sorting

1/31

encourage image

There's no tags or description

Looks like no tags are added yet.

Study Analytics
Name
Mastery
Learn
Test
Matching
Spaced

No study sessions yet.

32 Terms

1
New cards

What is social perception?

Social perception = “A general term for the processes by which people come to understand one another”.

Understanding arises from an interplay of 3 sources:

1. How we perceive the person

2. How we perceive the situation

3. How we perceive behaviour

2
New cards

A Person’s Physical Appearance

• People evaluate faces quickly, spontaneously, and unconsciously • We infer personal characteristics from the face– We read traits from faces, as well as read traits into faces, based on prior information • Superficial cues can lead us to form quick impressions

3
New cards

Confirmation bias

• The tendency to seek, interpret and create information that verifies existing beliefs. • This can lead to paying more attention to information about people that supports our first impressions, and overlooking contradictory information. • It can also lead to us making judgments about people based on our beliefs about the group they belong to.

4
New cards

Self-fulfilling prophecies

The process by which one’s expectations about a person eventually lead that person to behave in ways that confirm those expectations.

5
New cards

‘Pygmalion effect’ in teaching

• Teacher’s initial perceptions of their students significantly predict later student performance. • Is this an example of a self-fulfilling prophecy? Do teacher expectations influence their behaviour towards a student – thus influencing student performance? • Or are teachers just accurate judges not necessarily biased?

6
New cards

Target characteristics • Trait Negativity Bias

– Our impressions of people tend to be more influenced by negative information than positive information.– One negative trait may be enough to outweigh numerous positive traits.

7
New cards

Emotion and facial expressions

Charles Darwin (1872) “The young and the old of widely different races, both with man and animals, express the same state of mind by the same movements”. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals.

• The bio-evolutionary perspective argues that facial expressions of emotion are instinctive and universal, being either biologically innate (as the result of nervous system arousal) or having come about through evolutionary adaptation. • Based on initial work by Charles Darwin and Guillaume Duchenne (in one of the first scientific books to contain photographs).

8
New cards

Are emotions innate?

• In order to explore whether facial expressions of emotions are innate, rather than culturally learned, one would need to investigate the facial expressions used by humans that had had no contact with other humans. • The Fore people of the Okapu district in Papua New Guinea were one such uncontacted people until the 1950s. • The psychologist Paul Ekman visited the Fore in 1967/68 in an effort to find an answer to the question.

9
New cards

Nonverbal behaviour

Behaviour that reveals a person’s feelings without words, through facial expressions, body language and vocal cues.

10
New cards

Kinesics

• Kinesics: body movements and gestures as NVC • Ekman & Friesen (1969) defined five types of kinesics:– Emblems: gestures with a direct meaning e.g. ‘thumbs up’. These are highly culturally specific: in some cultures ‘thumbs up’ is an obscene gesture!

– Illustrators: body movements (usually with the hands) that accompany, and can accentuate speech

.– Adaptors/Manipulators: hair-touching, chin-stroking etc.

– Regulators: body movements that in some way regulate the flow of conversation e.g. nodding to show you understand, raising eyebrows to indicate surprise etc.

– Affective displays: Body movements that demonstrate emotio

11
New cards

Oculesics

• Oculesics: The use of the eyes in non-verbal communication • Eye contact or gaze: can be reassuring, or intimidating!

• Regulation of turn taking in conversation: looking at someone is a means of saying “I expect you to speak next”

• Affiliation: in groups, exchanging eye messages can be a way of expressing mutual agreement or disagreement with another e.g. eye rolling to indicate impatience/contempt

12
New cards

How do you know someone’s lying to you

Widespread belief that averting one’s gaze, or not looking someone in the eye, is the surest non-verbal sign that someone is lying (Vrij, 2008). • However, studies have shown that gaze aversion is not a significant indicator of lying behaviour.

Vrij (2008) suggests 2 reasons:

1. Making and maintaining eye contact is a well-practiced method of communication and thus easy to control.

2. Gaze is related to many other factors e.g. liking, embarrassment, power, proximity etc. Ironically, the belief that gaze aversion = deception may make it easier to get away with lying, simply by maintaining eye contact.

13
New cards

So what are signs of deception?

• Non-verbal behaviour accompanying lying is highly variable, contextual and dependent on perspective. There is no equivalent of Pinocchio's nose.

• However, DePaulo et al.’s meta-analysis (2003) did suggest the following NVC signs were suggestive of deception under certain circumstances: – Pupil dilation– Voice pitch– Lip-pressing– Blinking (but not in face-to-face interactions) longer to formulate answers.

• In general, verbal channels of communication are better indicators of deception than non-verbal channels: liars take

• There are also individual differences: you need a baseline of someone’s normal NVC when telling the truth to spot differences when they’re lying.

14
New cards

Contextual factors (Consequences-plannnig-complexitity- communication skills

• Consequences: Lies which are low-stakes and have few consequences (e.g. everyday social lies) are unlikely to provoke emotional difficulty or much cognitive effort. Telling high-stakes lies could be extremely stressful. • Planning: Liars who have had time to prepare a lie, may also have had time to prepare their self-presentation and anticipate the target’s reaction. effort to keep one’s story straight. • Complexity: The more complex the lie, the greater the cognitive • Communication skills: People used to self-presentation in everyday life can carry these skills over to deception: those with role-playing skills and non-verbal expressivity the most convincing (in other words, good actors!)

15
New cards

Attribution

The process of assigning a cause to our own behaviour and that of others.

16
New cards

Fritz Heider (1958) – The Psychology of interpersonal relationships

People are intuitive psychologists who construct causal theories of human behaviour.- We feel our own behaviour is motivated - In order to get through life, we develop a working understanding of why other people act the way they do.- We attribute intentions, motives and emotions to animate and inanimate figures.

17
New cards

Schemas

• A mental framework or body of knowledge that organises and synthesises information about something. • These help us interpret people, situations, events, roles, places etc

• Once we learn that in certain situations most people act in a specific way, we develop schemas for how we expect people to act in those situations. • Once we get to know a person, we develop schemas for how we expect them to act across situations.

18
New cards

Situational & dispositional factors

• Situational factors: stimuli in the environment • Dispositional factors: Individual personality characteristics.

Sometimes we attribute behaviour to external causes (situational factors) and sometimes to ‘what the person’s like’ (dispositional factors)

19
New cards

You arrive in the Tara building just in time for a 9am lecture. • The person going in the door in front of you doesn’t hold it for you but instead lets it slam in your face. • What’s your immediate reaction? Scenario • You may be inclined to make a dispositional attribution. • But consider:– Is the person who failed to hold the door the kind of person who normally fails to hold the door?– Or … do many (most) people fail to hold the door open when it’s 9am, and they’re trying to make a lecture?

20
New cards

Kelley’s covariation model of attribution

K • Harold H. Kelley (1921-2003). • Argued in his 1967 model of causal attribution that people make situational or dispositional attributions according to the covariation principle. • The covariation principle: Conditions that tend to be present when the event happens and absent when the event does not happen.

21
New cards

Theory of the week: Kelley’s theory of attribution

Three bases

Three bases through which we attribute a behaviour as having dispositional (internal) or situational (external) causes.

• Consensual behaviour: Would most people act like this in this situation?

• Consistency: Does this person normally act like this?

• Distinctiveness: Does this person only act like this in this particular situation?

22
New cards

Consensus

• If we see a behaviour, and think most people would act this way in the same situation, we make an external attribution. • If we think most people wouldn’t act this way in this situation, we make an internal attribution.

23
New cards

Consistency

• If we think a certain behaviour is typical of a specific individual, we make an internal attribution. • If we think a certain behaviour is atypical of a specific individual, we make an external attribution

24
New cards

Distinctiveness

• If we see someone behave in a specific way in one set of circumstances that is different to the way they behave in most other sets of circumstances, we make an external attribution. • If someone seems to behave in a similar specific way in most sets of circumstances, we make an internal attribution

<p>• If we see someone behave in a specific way in one set of circumstances that is different to the way they behave in most other sets of circumstances, we make an external attribution. • If someone seems to behave in a similar specific way in most sets of circumstances, we make an internal attribution</p>
25
New cards

Errors & biases in attribution

• Kelley’s theory of attribution provides us with a way of mapping how people make decisions about situational and dispositional causes of behaviour. • However, people take shortcuts, resulting in errors and bias. • We are particularly likely to do this in occasions where we are under time pressure, or where we are not motivated to think things through carefully.

Cognitive heuristics: Information-processing rules of thumb that enable us to think in ways that are quick and easy, but often lead to error.

<p> • Kelley’s theory of attribution provides us with a way of mapping how people make decisions about situational and dispositional causes of behaviour. • However, people take shortcuts, resulting in errors and bias. • We are particularly likely to do this in occasions where we are under time pressure, or where we are not motivated to think things through carefully. </p><p>Cognitive heuristics: Information-processing rules of thumb that enable us to think in ways that are quick and easy, but often lead to error.</p>
26
New cards

Fundamental attribution error

r • We tend to overestimate the importance of dispositional explanations rather than situational ones i.e. we tend to assume that behaviour is reflective of someone’s personality rather than the context. • This tends to happen even in circumstances where there’s compelling evidence that something about the situation caused the behaviour. • This appears to be an artefact of more individualist Western societies – it is not as apparent in non-Western cultures. – Furthermore, it would appear to be learnt. Young children tend to favour situational rather than dispositional explanations.– In other words, this is not innate. It is the result of socialisation in individualist societies.

27
New cards

The actor-observer effect

• We make different types of observations for our own and other’s behaviour. • In most circumstances, we are more likely to see our own behaviour as influenced by the situation, and the behaviour of others as dispositional

28
New cards

Self-serving bias

• We tend to attribute our successes to internal factors and our failures to external factors. • Making attributions in this way protects and maintains our self-esteem

Self-serving bias at a group level • We tend to attribute in-group failures and out-group successes to external causes such as luck. • We tend to attribute in-group success and out-group failures to internal causes such as properties of the group and their members

29
New cards

False consensus

• The tendency for people to overestimate the extent to which others share their opinions, attributes and behaviours. • This can lead us to make incorrect attributions based on ‘most people would behave this way in this situation’. • Likely to occur if we surround ourselves with people who behave and think in the same way we do.

30
New cards

Social Perception: The Bottom Line

• Dual-process theory: Two ways of forming social perceptions 1. Quickly and relatively automatically • Based on physical appearance, preconceptions, cognitive heuristics, with minimal behavioral evidence

2. Mindfully, with control situation scenario.

• Based on careful observation and logical analysis of the individual, behavior, and

• Both approaches may be subject to bias: but more likely in the first

• Experience leads to more accurate judgements.

• Seeking to control for potential biases, and being aware of situational factors informing behaviour is an important facet of critical thinking.

31
New cards

Attribution and relationship success

• Attributional conflict: where partners have divergent causal interpretations of behaviours and disagree over what attributions to adopt. • Happy couples tend to credit their partners with internal attributions for positive behaviours and explain away negative behaviours through external attributions. • Unhappy couples do the opposite.

32
New cards

Correspondent inference theory

• Under what circumstances does an expressed attitude correspond with an underlying opinion?– How do we know that someone means what they say? • Highlighted two main elements– Perceived choice– Prior probability

We are most likely to attribute an expressed statement with an underlying belief when…– The statement is unexpected/unpopular– The statement appears to have been made as a free choice.

look at quiz show experiment in week 3 tutorial.