1/39
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
Emotions
are feelings or affect triggered in situations that are important to you.
Amygdala
is the brain structure, especially in the limbic system, most involved in emotion.
Prefrontal cortex
Maturation of the allows for improved emotional regulation and communication.
Six
There are universally experienced basic emotions: happy, anger, surprise, disgust, sadness, and fear.
Blood pressure
Basic emotions involve subjective feelings and physiological changes such as increased .
Voice
Overt behaviors expressing emotions include facial expressions and tone of .
Smiles
At around 1 month, infants display reflexive that may be true or reflexive.
Social
Between 2 and 3 months, infants begin to show smiles directed at people.
Anger
By 4–6 months, infants can express the emotion of .
Fear
Around 6 months, infants commonly display the emotion of .
Stranger
Distress in the presence of an unfamiliar adult is called anxiety.
Self
Self‑conscious emotions such as pride and guilt require development of a sense of .
Seven
There are complex self‑conscious emotions including pride, guilt, embarrassment, empathy, despair, and jealousy.
Words
Understanding self‑conscious emotions is aided by learning to label abstract reactions.
States
Parents talking with children about mental improves their understanding of emotions.
Nine
The ability to experience regret and relief typically develops around age .
Restraint
In Asian cultures, emotional is favored over overt displays of emotion.
Sad
By 6 months, infants can differentiate happy faces from faces.
Afraid
In social referencing, a 12‑month‑old is less likely to play with a new toy if their caregiver appears .
Annoying
By 18 months, infants weigh conflicting information, playing less when an adult describes a toy as .
Looking
Infants regulate their emotions by thumb sucking, using a security blanket, or away from unpleasant scenes.
Soothing
A two‑way communication system for emotion regulation involves infant distress signals followed by caregiver .
Regulation
Diverting attention to less emotional stimuli is an example of cognitive .
Easy
The temperament category for infants who adjust easily and follow routines is called the child.
Difficult
Infants who react intensely and have irregular routines are called children.
Surgency
Rothbart’s temperament dimension involving happiness, activity, and seeking stimulation is called extraversion or .
Affect
Rothbart’s dimension characterized by anger, fear, frustration, and shyness is called negative .
Effortful
Rothbart’s dimension involving focused attention and inhibitory control is known as control.
Heredity
Greater similarity in temperament among monozygotic versus dizygotic twins indicates a role for .
Goodness
The concept of how well a child’s temperament matches environmental demands is called of fit.
DRD4
Variants of the gene influence attention, motivation, and novelty‑seeking in temperament.
Inhibition
Kagan’s term for initial avoidance or distress to unfamiliar stimuli is behavioural .
Pre‑attachment
Bowlby’s stage from birth to 2 months, when infants recognize their mother’s smell and sound, is called .
Secure
In Ainsworth’s Strange Situation, infants upset when the caregiver leaves but soothed on return demonstrate attachment.
Disorganized
Infants who appear confused and disoriented during caregiver separation and return are classified as attachment.
Oxytocin
The hormone supports maternal behaviors and is released during birth and breastfeeding.
Available
Secure infants tend to have caregivers who are responsive and .
Unavailable
Avoidant attachment often results from caregivers who are rejecting or .
Secure
Adults who describe childhood experiences objectively and value caregiver relationships are classified as adults.
Play
Unstructured, unsupervised allows children to experiment, regulate emotions, and problem‑solve.