1/61
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
Foveal Vision
Area of retina with highest photoreceptor density; provides sharpest vision
Peripheral Vision
Region outside the fovea with lower visual acuity
Retinal Angle
Unit (in degrees) for measuring visual space on the retina
Cortical Magnification
Foveal input is disproportionately processed in V1 and beyond
Physical eye structure and acuity
Fixation Duration
Eye remains still for 250–300ms to process visual input
Saccade
Rapid, ballistic eye movement (up to 700°/second) between fixations
Saccadic Suppression
Temporary inhibition of visual input during saccades
Reading Skipping
Only ~⅔ of words when reading for comprehension
Regression
Backward eye movement to previously read text, regressions (10–15% of fixations)
Four different metrics
can be measured to explore different experimental effects:
1.Fixation Duration Metric
Indicates processing difficulty per word or phrase
2.Regression Rate
Percentage of backward fixations during reading
3.Word Skipping Rate
Frequency of skipped words during reading
4.Saccade Distance/speed
Length/speed of eye movement between fixations
Perceptual Span
Number of characters processed per fixation; depends on language and direction. When you read, your eyes don’t move smoothly — they jump in quick movements (called saccades) and stop briefly (called fixations). During each fixation, you don’t just "see" the word you’re looking at — you also take in some information to the left and right of it. That visible area is your perceptual span
Gaze-Contingent
Display changes stimulus based on real-time eye position (where the reader is fixating)
Hebrew Perceptual Span
(read right-to-left), the perceptual span is the same size as in English, but flipped — readers see more to the left (ahead in that direction).
Japanese Perceptual Span
adjust based on layout: when reading left-to-right, they see about 13 characters to the right; when reading top-to-bottom, the span shifts 6 characters downward
Chinese Perceptual Span
1 character left, 3 right (Inhoff & Liu, 1998)
Text Difficulty and Span
Perceptual span changes with text complexity (Rayner, 1986) wider for easier texts and narrower when text is more challenging
Eye Tracking: Scleral Search Coil Tracking
Uses current shifts from a coil to track eye movement (<0.1° error)
EOG (Electrooculography)
Electrodes detect eye movement via changes in electrical field
Dual Purkinje Image Tracker
Uses infrared reflection to track fine eye movements, combination of lenses and mirrors reflect this light showing any movements made
EyeLink Eye Tracker
Infrared light illuminates the eye, the corneal reflection is monitored using a camera that samples the location of the eye at 1000Hz
Tobii Eye Tracker
Tracks eye movement at 90–120Hz and allows for head movement
Word Frequency Effect
High-frequency words are recognised faster (e.g., 67ms faster in eye-tracking). Up to 300ms difference in lexical decision task and 67ms in eye movement in reading studies
Measures of word frequency
Kucera-Francis Corpus: only 1,014 words from sources that are not the most typical of our language use. SUBTLEX: subtitles of 10,000 films and TV programmes and features 51 million words
Predictability Effect
Contextually expected words (e.g., "birds") are read ~80ms faster
Word Length Effect
Longer words are processed more slowly (~88ms longer)
Phonological Preview Benefit
Phonological information (how a word sounds) influences how we read and recognise words. Previewing a homophone (e.g., pair before pear) speeds recognition by about 20 ms (showing we automatically sound while reading). We use sound-based similarity to support word recognition
Automatic Phonological Access
Phonological code is automatically accessed during reading
Phonological Neighbourhood
Larger neighbourhoods lead to quicker recognition
Phonological Neighbours
Words differing by one phoneme (e.g., “pit” vs “bit”) improve recognition speed… some letters (like first and last) are more important than others in recognising words, we don’t treat all letter positions equally
Syntactic Parsing
mental process of assigning roles (subject, object, verb) to sentence parts; errors cause garden-path effects
Garden Path Sentence
Misleading syntax forces sentence reanalysis (“The horse raced…”), trick the reader because the initial interpretation turns out to be wrong. We make assumptions when parsing syntactic structure
Reanalysis
when we realise our initial parsing was wrong (as in garden path sentences), we reanalyse the sentence to find the correct structure
Incremental Interpretation
Sentence meaning is built word-by-word during reading (don’t wait till the end of the sentence), readers semantically interpret and syntactically parse text on a word-by-word basis
Minimal Attachment Principle
readers try to interpret sentences with the simplest possible syntactic structure
Incremental interpretation, visual world paradigm
eye movements are tracked while participants listen to narrative
Semantic Plausibility
Readers quickly detect semantic anomalies (e.g., “knife to chop carrots”), readers immediately detect semantic anomaly suggesting we incrementally interpret semantic aspects. Implausible (“used a pump to inflate the carrots”)… however can overridden by context (thanks pragmatics!)
Contextual Override
Implausible phrases can be accepted if context supports them (“mouse lit dynamite”)
Semantic Memory in Reading
Words are accessed based on associative semantic meaning
Model of Word Recognition
rely on the notion of having a mental lexicon (‘internal dictionary’). A store of all lexical representations, with their semantic meaning, syntactic role and phonology encoded within these representations. Not a single neural structure attributed to language knowledge
Interactive Activation Model
Higher frequency words are said to have ‘lower threshold’ for activation, more predictable words are said to be ‘primed’ so they have already received from activation, hence the threshold is reached quicker. This is also the explanation for semantic and phonological priming
Dual Route Cascade Model
Two pathways:
Route 1: Grapheme-phoneme conversion
Grapheme is the visual unit that corresponds to a phoneme (i in “pig”, igh in “high”). Conversion rules used to convert each grapheme into a phoneme. Rules determined by the most common grapheme-phoneme association in the language. Good for regular words and non-words, bad for irregular words!
Route 2: Lexicon + semantics
Orthographic input lexicon stores the spelling of all the words you know. Activates meaning and/or phonology (hence semantic and phonological priming effects). Good for reading all familiar words, bad for reading unfamiliar words (hence frequency and length effects)
Connectionist Triangle Model
Links orthography to phonology directly or via semantics. 1. Direct pathway from orthography to phonology. 2. Indirect pathway from orthography to phonology via semantics
EEG in Reading Research
Tracks brain electrical activity during reading; excellent temporal, poor spatial resolution
Self-Paced Reading Method
Measures reading time by button press; lacks natural reading behaviours
Lexical Decision Task
Participants judge whether a string is a real word; reveals recognition, not comprehension
Questionnaire Method
Gathers subjective data on reading experience or comprehension
Corpus Analysis
Studies word patterns and frequency using large-scale text databases
Corpus Choice in Frequency Effects
Word frequency effects differ by corpus (e.g., SUBTLEX vs. Kucera-Francis)
Web Reading Challenges
Navigation and scrolling may alter reading patterns
Language Generalisability
Effects found in English may not apply to other scripts like Chinese
Semantics in Models
Many models underemphasise semantics, reducing their explanatory power
Speed Reading Claims
Apps may boost word recognition, but not necessarily comprehension
Environmental Influences
Factors like caffeine may impact eye movement and reading behaviour