14: acquisition of passives

0.0(0)
studied byStudied by 0 people
learnLearn
examPractice Test
spaced repetitionSpaced Repetition
heart puzzleMatch
flashcardsFlashcards
Card Sorting

1/22

encourage image

There's no tags or description

Looks like no tags are added yet.

Study Analytics
Name
Mastery
Learn
Test
Matching
Spaced

No study sessions yet.

23 Terms

1
New cards

why do children acquire passives late

  • not sure if due to difficulties with movement or other properties of passives like:

    • input

    • order of NPs

    • presence/ absence of by-phrase

    • type of passive (agent and patient semantically reversible or not; verb is actional or non-actional)

2
New cards

thematic roles in passives

  • subject of passive is the theme/patient rather than the agent

  • the agent is optionally expressed in a prepositional phrase (PP)(‘by-phrase’): ex. the apple was eaten (by the girl)

3
New cards

short passive

  • no by-phrase; the agent is not expressed

    • ex. the apple was eaten

    • ex. Jamie was kicked

4
New cards

what is a long-passive

  • optional by-phrase is expressed;

    • ex. the apple was eaten by the girl

    • ex. Jamie was kicked by Robin

5
New cards
<p>where are short phrases derived from</p>

where are short phrases derived from

  • theme originates in object position

  • theme moves to subject position

6
New cards

deriving long passives

  • agent is base-generated in a PP inside the VP

  • the PP is an adjunct not an argument (it gets ‘demoted’ (lowered rank position))

    • adjunction provide additional info that is not needed to complete the meaning of a predicate

  • agent cannot be base generated in a grammatical subject position since the agent is not obligatory

<ul><li><p><strong>agent</strong> is base-generated in a<strong> PP inside the VP</strong></p></li><li><p>the PP is an <strong>adjunct </strong>not an argument (it gets ‘demoted’ (lowered rank position))</p><ul><li><p>adjunction provide additional info that is not needed to complete the meaning of a predicate</p></li></ul></li><li><p>agent cannot be base generated in a grammatical subject position since <strong>the agent is not obligatory</strong></p></li></ul><p></p>
7
New cards

what are the acquisition of passives impacted by (challenges):

  • order of NPs : non-canonical (agent =/= subject; theme =/= object)

  • input: passive are rare in child-directed speech in many languages (including English)

  • by-phrase: presence vs absence of by-phrase

  • type of passive: reversible vs non-reversible; actional vs non-actional

8
New cards

evidence in input

English

  • only 0.4% of verbal utterances in CDS are passive constructions

  • English children fail to comprehend some types of passives until 9 years of age

Sesotho

  • 2.7% of verbal utterances in CDS are passive constructions;

  • Sesotho children comprehend all types of passives by 3;1.

  • Sesotho passives are more frequent in the input and they are used in more discourse contexts than English passives.

9
New cards

reversibility vs animacy in passives

  • subjects are typically animate and agents of actions (volitionally causing an event or change of state

  • subject and object are semantically reversible when both are animate :

    • ex. Jamie was kicked by Robin

    • Robin was kicked by Jamie

      • challenge: who is doing the kicking

  • subject and object are not (typically) reversible when only one is animate:

    • ex. the apple was eaten by the girl

      • less challenging: the girl is animate; apple is inanimate

10
New cards

actional vs non-actional verbs

  • actional verbs denote actions or events that are observable; the theme/patient is affected by the event:

    • ex. the apple was eaten

    • ex. jamie was kicked

  • Non-actional verbs denote a state or something less visible; includes verbs of perception or emotion:

    • ex. the boy was seen

    • ex. Sarah was remembered

11
New cards

de Villiers & de Villiers act-out study (1973)

  • 33 children, aged 19-38 months;

  • Calculated MLU for each child (it proved to be a better predictor for behaviour than age);

  • Act out task: Child provided with toys and/or props; experimenter asks the child to act out a sentence;

  • Stimuli involved reversible passive and active sentences, all with actional verbs:

    • ex. the horse was pushed by the donkey

    • the horse pushed the donkey

12
New cards

results from de Villiers & de Villiers study (1973) at MLU 1.5

  • Low accuracy with reversible passive and reversible active sentences

  • Errors:

    • Children place themselves as the agent:
      The horse was pushed by the donkey. (act out: child pushes donkey)

    • Children reverse the arguments:
      The horse was pushed by the donkey. (act out: child has horse push donkey)

13
New cards

results from de Villiers & de Villiers study (1973) at MLU 1.5-3

  • Children act out reversible active sentences well: 80+% accuracy;

  • Still do poorly with reversible passives: ~30% accuracy.

14
New cards

results from de Villiers & de Villiers study (1973) at MLU 3.5-4.25

  • Some improvement with reversible passives but still quite low: ~40% accuracy.

  • The oldest children in this study are 3;2 – children perform a lot better by age 4

15
New cards

what do findings on non-actionable verbs show

  • English-learning children have problems with non-actional passive constructions until about the age of 9

  • Performance on non-actional verbs gets better when stimuli do not contain a by-phrase

16
New cards

maratos et al. (1985) study

  • 80 children, aged 4-11;

  • Picture identification task to test comprehension of passives with actional verbs (wash, kiss, push, kick) and non-actional verbs (see, hear, love, fear); active sentences also included;

  • The child is shown two pictures for each verb. The experimenter says a sentence and the child has to point to the correct picture

    • ex. sentence with actional verb: Mickey is held by Batman.

    • with non-actional verb: Mickey is seen by Batman.

17
New cards

results— meratsos study

  • Accuracy on actional passives is high even for youngest group (4-year-olds);

  • Accuracy on non-actional passives is low; above-chance performance is not observed until age 9!

<ul><li><p><strong>Accuracy on actional passives is high</strong> even for <strong>youngest</strong> <strong>group</strong> (4-year-olds);</p></li><li><p>Accuracy on<strong> non-actional passives is low;</strong> above-chance performance is <strong>not observed until age 9!</strong></p></li></ul><p></p>
18
New cards

Fox and Godzinsky (1998) study: Do children’s performance in non-actional passive improve without the by-phrase?

  • Goal is to test whether children’s performance on non-actional passives improves without the by-phrase.

  • In such sentences, there is no competition between the two NPs, both of which could be agents.

  • 13 children, aged 3;6 to 5;5.

  • Truth value judgement task: Child listens to short story accompanied by pictures. Puppet who is “learning language” summarizes story at end – correctly or incorrectly. Child says whether puppet was correct or incorrect

19
New cards

results of Fox & Grodzinsky (1998) study

  • more accurate responses with short passives than with long passives (by-phrases)

    • ex. The boy is seen. (≈85% accuracy)

    • The boy is seen by the horse. (≈45% accuracy)

  • Interpretation: Children’s difficulty with passives involves understanding how the agent is expressed in a by-phrase, not the movement of the object.

20
New cards

spontaneous production

Evidence for PRODUCTIVITY of passives (knowing how they work) in spontaneous production: children create novel passives, suggesting that their grammars do permit passive constructions

  • It was bandaided. (age 3;4)
    Is it all needled. (age 3;2)
    I don’t like being fallen down on. (age 4)

  • When producing passives, children correctly move the object: “the puppy got washed” → *got washed the puppy

  • however, long passives are rarely produced spontaneously (i.e. without being prompted)

21
New cards

elicited production task

4-year old children were taught novel verbs (through demonstration of actions).

Training phase:

• Verbs were presented to children in either active or passive voice;

  • Example: they would learn that gomp means ' to back into’.

Testing phase:

• To elicit production of passive, children heard a story that provided a context for the use of passive with the novel verb.

• The child is expected to say either:

  1. The elephant is being gomped (by the tiger), or

  2. The tiger is gomping the elephant

Passive (a) is expected because the tiger hasn’t been mentioned yet

22
New cards

effective production task results

  • Children used more actives than passives in their responses;

  • This is not surprising because passives were not required in such cases;

  • Adults also tend to prefer actives in similar tasks.

  • However:

  • Passives were sometimes produced by the children;

  • This happened regardless of whether or not the child had been trained on the active or passive forms of the verb;

  • This suggests productive knowledge of passive constructions;

  • In a subsequent task, children were tested via act-out task on comprehension of the novel verbs (e.g., ‘can you make it so that the elephant is gomped by the tiger’) and showed above-chance comprehension of passives.

23
New cards

are passives ever acquired early on in production

  • Early production of passives is more common in languages where passives are required in many discourse contexts;

  • Example: In Sesotho, the questioning of subjects requires passives:

  • Di-jo di-pheh-w-a ke mang? 8-food 8agr-cook-PASS-FV by 1-who ‘The food is being cooked by who?’

  • Findings:
    2-year-old children
    produce passives regularly and correctly (Demuth 1989).