1/38
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
Three core executive functions
(Miyake et al., 2000)
Inhibition
Controlling attention/behaviour/thoughts and/or emotion to override a prepotent response
Working Memory
Holding information in mind whilst simultaneously processing it
Cognitive Flexibility
Changing perspectives/approaches to a problem, flexibly adjusting to new demands/rules/priorities.
Development of inhibitory control - childhood
Very difficult for children
Rapid Growth in early childhood
Wiebe et al. (2012)
3-6 years
Button press response inhibition task
Accuracy increased by 30%
Development of inhibitory control - early adulthood
Ordaz et al., (2016)
Longitudinal data - growth curve modelling
Continued maturation of error-processing abilities
Supports protracted development of inhibitory control over adolescence
Working memory development - Infancy
Kibbe & Leslie (2016; 2013)
A WMC of 1 item at 6 months → 3-4 items by the end of 1st year
PROBLEM: Tasks for infants cannot provide comparable data to children and adult data
Working memory development - School-aged children
Roome et al. (2014; 2019)
Capacity of 1.5-2.5 items
Working memory development - Cowan et al.
Takes a long time to develop
Measuring development of cognitive flexibility
Zelazo et al. (1996): Dimensional Change Card Sort Test
One dimensional version of Wisconsin Card Sorting Task
4yo: can sort by colour or shape, but cannot switch from one dimension to another
7-21 yo: shift difficulty decreased with each age group, but 15yo did not differ from adults (Huizinga et al., 2006)
Ferguson et al., (2021)
Compared developmental trajectories of EFs
Controlled for IQ and SES (environmental factors)
Inhibition and WM show similar developmental trajectories
Theodoraki et al., (2020)
Found correlations between:
Inhibition and Color Naming Speed
Inhibition and Working Memory
Shifting Measures (3 & 4)
Measuring development via. factor loading
3 different tests:
7-21 yo: 2 FL (Huizinga et al., 2006)
7-14 yo 3 FL (Wu et al., 2011)
8-13 yo: 4 FL (Agostino et al., 2010)
Criticisms of Fractionation of executive functioning
Too many tasks testing the same concept
Tasks are complex and multi-dimensional
Difficult to pinpoint the specific skills being assessed
Conceptual ambuiguity
Is perserveration due to inability to inhibit or inability to switch rules?
Study criticising fractionation of executive functioning
Switches in which EFS predict behaviour
Tower of Hanoi - Senn et al. (2004)
Younger than 4 → predicted by inhibition
Older than 4 → predicted by WM
Maturation of the whole brain
Gogtay et al. (2004)
Maturation at age 5 vs 20
Structural prefrontal cortex (PFC) development - Progressive changes
Myelination,
Neuron proliferation,
Synaptogenesis
Structural prefrontal cortex (PFC) development - Regressive changes
Cell death,
Synaptic pruning,
Loss in grey matter.
Perseveration
The involuntary repetition of a behavior, speech, or thought that continues even after the original stimulus has changed or ended
Neural proliferation
The process of generating new brain cells, or neurons, during embryonic development
Synaptogenesis
The formation of synapses that occurs throughout an individual’s lifespan, occurs most during early childhood.
Cell death
A natural process that occurs when cells that are not used stop working and die.
Synaptic pruning
The process in which the brain removes neurons and synapses that it does not need.
Usually takes place between 2–10 years old
Grey matter loss
Occurs as a result of myelination and synaptic pruning
Volume decreases but density increases
Maturation of the prefrontal cortex in infancy (12-18 months)
(Huttenlocher et al., 1997)
Peak in synaptogenesis in the middle frontal gyrus
Maturation of the prefrontal cortex in infancy (6-12 months)
Koenderink etal. (1994)
Increases in dendritic trees in layer III of the PFC
Maturation of the prefrontal cortex in infancy - (childhood vs adulthood)
(Glantz et al., 2007)
Synaptogenesis in the PFC vs reduction in synapses
Cerebral blood volume (CBV)
Franceschini et al. (2007)
Frontal region: increases linearly from birth to 8-9 months of age.
Occipital, parietal and temporal regions: Exponential increase seen only from birth to 2.5 months
Spread of myelination
Deoni et al. (2011)
1st year of life: cerebellum, pons, internal capsule
4-6 months: occipital and parietal lobes
6-8 months: frontal and temporal lobes
remember: back to front
Grey matter volume
Giedd et al (1999)
Increases in frontal and parietal lobes
Peak 10yo in girls, 12yo in boys
Followed by decreases in these lobes
Grey matter density
Gogtay et al (2004) - Longitudinal data
Loss starting at puberty
In the sensorimotor areas
Then spreading over the frontal cortex (rostrally) and the parietal
Then temporal cortex (caudally)
DLPFC loses grey matter last
Maturation rate of the PFC
Structural and metabolic changes in the first years of life
Relatively late maturation in comparison to other brain regions
Connectivity evident within pre-school years
Difficulty studying structural development - Holmboe et al (2018)
Individual differences
Variations in attentional control at 6 months were predictive of inhibitory control abilities at 12 months
Aligns with Deoni et al., (2011)
Difficulty studying structural development - Baird et al. (2002)
Brain scans often require children to stay still, where as NIRS doesn’t.
Found that 5-12 month-old infants recruit the DL-PFC when holding objects in visual WM.
Difficulty studying structural development - Mehnert et al. (2013)
Go/NoGo task in 4-6yo and adults
Adults: R frontal and parietal regions during inhibition (no-go)
Children: R frontal and parietal regions during go, and no-go
Task may not be appropriate for children
Difficulty studying structural development - Moriguchi et al. (2009; 2011)
3 yo who could switch in the DCCS task showed a bilateral increase in oxygenation in the ventral and dorsolateral PFC.
Individual differences in cognitive flexibility
Functional connectivity
How different areas of the brain communicate and work together, during specific tasks or at rest
Functional connectivity of the PFC in infants
Ezekiel et al., (2013) - fMRI
Weaker between the lateral pFC, ACC, inferior parietal cortex
At 3yo: Weak neural interactions within the frontal cortex and unrefined frontoparietal connectivity
At 4.5yo: Stronger neural connections and a refined pathways with efficient connectivity
Conclusion of Ezekiel et al., (2013)
Confirms the involvement of the PFC in early EF
Shows PFC does not work in isolation
Quantitative changes in functional connectivity - Englehardt et al. (2019)
Compared the location of the adult regions of interest (ROI) to the children’s common task activity
Switching, updating and inhibition tasks
Taken the centre of the clusters
Child samples were within 15mm of the adult-based ROIs
Qualitative and quantitative changes of functional connectivity - Moisala et al. (2018)
2 tasks: Attention and WM tasks (division of attention, inhibition distractors, WM and attentional switching)
13-14 yo: brain regions unique to one task, connectivity is spread
20-24 yo: brain regions active for all tasks, connectivity is short and dense
Importance of executive functions
Mental and physical health, quality of life, school readiness,
school and job success, marital harmony, public safety.