1/30
week 6 content
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
What is spoken communication?
Spoken communication is interaction with others, relationships, friendships, education, literacy, qualifications, employment and quality of life.
Parent reactions
Grief at different levels
acute initially for some- immediately obvious
less acute as a slow realisation of a problem is revealed
chronic and on going
not the life I had imagined
Parent self talk
Guilt
Sense of failure
Inadequacy
what are the implications for child and family
Interactions with other toddlers are delayed
less vocal interaction- less social interaction
less social interaction- less opportunity for language development'
less language- reduced social interaction Cyclical pattern
Frustration emerges and distressed behaviours emerge.
commence SLT using developmental sequence to interventions
Implications for family
Increased stress because of child’s frustration and distressed behaviours
Parents can become anxious
What is the role of professionals
Listen and support
provide realistic hope
refrain from judgement
provide honest comments
seek out the positives
never under estimate the challenges
empathise and imagine how it would be to walk in their shoes
provide useful strategies and activities
show families how to perceive and celebrate small gains
ensure families know that you value and respect their child
consequences of communication difficulties in children
increasing evidence that communication difficulties in children are a life-long disability
often a additional diagnostic label is used (ASD, ADHD, FASD, behavioural problems) many of these conditions have a communication difficulty at their core or as a associated factor
Evidence for consequences
Clegg et al (2005)
assessed long term outcomes of adults with a history of severe receptive developmental language disorder (DLD) diagnosed in childhood.
participants: 17 adult men mid 30s diagnosed with severe receptive DLD in childhood
comparative groups:
non language disordered siblings
adults matched on age and non verbal IQ
and a broad national cohort matched on IQ and social class.
findings of Clegg et al (2005)
Individuals with childhood DLD showed significantly worse social adaptation in adulthood.
Specific issues included:
History of unemployment
Limited close friendships
Few or failed romantic relationships
Frequent relationship breakdowns
Conclusion:
Severe receptive DLD in childhood is linked to long-term social and occupational difficulties in adulthood, even when controlling for IQ, social class, and family background.
What did their childhood receptive language impairment (still in adulthood) involve significant deficits in?
theory of mind
verbal short term memory
phonological processing
substantial social adaptation difficulties
increased risk of psychiatric disorder in adult life
what was the aim of the Johnson et al (2010) study
This study was a community sample of adults with 112 and without 113 early speech and or language impairments
What did the Johnson et al study find
at age 25, those with a history of language impairments showed- poorer outcomes in multiple domains (communication, cognition, educational attainment, and occupational status) than their peers without early communication impairments
They did not differ in: subjective perceptions of their quality of life from the other groups
subjective well being was primarily associated with strong networks of family, friends, others.
What was the aim of Elbro et al (2011)
A 30 year follow up of 198 participants who had been diagnosed with language impairments at 3-9 years of age.
what did the Elbro et al study find
most of the participants reported literacy difficulties, unemployment, and low socio economic status (at rates significantly higher than in the general population)
Participants diagnosed with specific language impairment (SLI) as a child had significantly better outcomes than those with additional diagnoses. (e.g. ASD, ADHD) irrespective of their non-verbal IQ
Trembath et al (2021)
the relationship between language difficulties, psychosocial difficulties and speech language pathology service access in the community.
Secondary analysis of previous collected data. Total of 808 children who completed a language assessment
The findings point to the possible under identification of children with language difficulties in the community and the need for clinical service provision that accounts for the multi faced needs that these children are likely to have
Cohen et al (1998) on behavioural problems
This study examined 7-14 year olds with specific language impairment
children previously identified as having language impairments were more likely to be diagnosed as having ADHD.
were rated by both parents and teachers as more socially withdrawn.
Lindsay and Dockrell (2000) on behavioural problems
studied 7-8 year old children with specific language impairment
more likely to have behavioural problems than their non-language impaired peers, and boys were rated more severe than girls; especially with hyperactivity and conduct disorders
parent ratings of behaviour was more severe than teacher’s ratings
self esteem scores were not different to peers
Kedge (2007)
assessed 34 children referred to a behaviour service (NZ schools) at ages 8-14 years.
33 had never had a referral for language difficulties
19 scored below the normal range for age on a test oof oral language (56%)
all but 2 scored within normal limits on a test of non-verbal intelligence.
What is the relationship with the criminal justice system
Research over 10-15 years in the UK, US, Australia, and NZ
it has been long known that levels of literacy in the incarcerated population are well below average.
“screening of 120 people in NZ prisons by literacy expert found that nearly half had significant dyslexia. (52% men, 43% women), previously undiagnosed. More than 80% had been at secondary school for 2 years or less, with many having been excluded in their first year.” (Lambie 2020)
Summary of Lambie (2022) report ‘A breakdown across the whole system’
This follow up report is based on “how we fail children who offend'“ and what to do and the need for speech language therapy in children as a preventative factor for children who are at risk for becoming young offenders because of their language delay.
Bryan et al (2007) UK study
assessed the language and communication skills 15-17 year olds in a juvenile offenders institution (N=58)
90% had ceased to attend school before age 16
66-90% had below average language skills with 46-67% poor or very poor
Snow and Powell (2008) AUS study
50 juvenile offenders and 50 controls (mean age 15.8 yrs)
measured on language processing and production, social skill.
and IQ (K-bit)
– Young offenders performed significantly worse on all language
and social skill measures, but not on non-verbal IQ
– 50% self-reported reading and writing difficulties in the early
school years cf. 20% of controls
– 61.5% had received some form of early intervention (e.g.
Reading Recovery), cf. 29% of the controls
– Just over half of the young offenders were identified as
language impaired
Lount et al 2017 NZ study
33 young people, 36 matched controls, 14-18 years
– Significantly different on all language measures
– Both groups were within normal limits on the Test of
Nonverbal Intelligence
– 11% of the control participants and 64% of youth
offenders met criteria for language impairment
– Similar to the UK and Australian studies. No reason
to think it is better here, but there are cultural
differences. We need local research and local
solutions
What is the Aotearoa context
>50% of young people in youth courts in NZ are Māori
None of the language assessments available here were normed or designed for Māori
Kedge and McCann study
Small scale unpublished report
– 21 young people in a youth justice residence in NZ (mean
age 16.2 yrs) measured on receptive and expressive
language (CELF-4), “time concepts”, understanding legal
vocabulary. Also asked for their own views about talking,
listening and understanding
– 19% scored within the average range
– 33% scored below the normal range (mild language
impairment)
– 47% scored well below normal range (moderate or severe
language impairment)
factors associated with communication disorders
Neurodevelopmental issues are likely (e.g., communication
disorders and ADHD and hearing loss)
– More likely to have acquired issues than their peers; head
injury, addictions (drug & alcohol abuse), poor nutrition
– Very likely to have grown up with social or socio-economic
issues; poverty, struggling parenting, domestic abuse &
trauma, unemployment, intergenerational post-colonial
trauma, racism, frequent moves/transitions, little stability
physically or emotionally
– Poor educational achievement, gang involvement
– Lambie (2020) “Higher rates [of all of these] in justice-
involved people
Impact on social relationships and friends
Friendships and social relationships are facilitated through
communication (in many different forms)
• Studies have become very interested in the impact of
communication difficulties on friendships
Durkin & Conti-Ramsden (2007) in UK
– compared friendship quality in 16-year-old adolescents with
(120) and without (118) specific language impairment
– language measures were associated with friendship quality
– typically developing adolescents enjoyed normal
friendships, whereas those with language impairment were
more likely to exhibit poorer quality friendships
• McCormack et al (2010) in Australia
– 7-9 year olds reported more bullying, poorer peer
relationships and less enjoyment of school than their peers
Impact on cognition and language
Many cognitive measures are confounded by language
– ‘verbal IQ’ is part of the total measure of IQ and depends on
verbal questions and responses
– Can measure IQ nonverbally (‘performance IQ’, TONI etc)
– As children with communication difficulties grow older, their
non-verbal IQ measures appear to decrease
• Probably reflects the increase in verbal input to non-verbal
skill performance that occurs over time
strong and weak cognitive hypotheses
Language depends on cognition, or is related but
not dependent
– Thought precedes language (Piaget)
– Cognition is needed for some language skills, but
not all
– Partial mismatch of language and cognition (Rice;
2000)
– Some go as far as seeing language and cognition as
having separate bases, with some interconnections
(Langacker, 1968, Vygotsky, 1962)
cognitive processes required for higher level language of reasoning and inferencing
Figurative language
• Ambiguity, multiple meanings, jokes
• Imagination – ‘inner language’
• Information processing – including selection and
attention and memory processes
• Executive function “regulating feelings and behaviours to
select and guide behaviours to follow rules and attain
goals”
• These are all sites of difficulty in language disorders in
children (and adults), especially from school-age onwards