1/108
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
Clark Doll Test
1940s study by Kenneth & Mamie Clark where Black children often preferred white dolls, showing internalized racism and its impact on self-esteem. Later used in Brown v. Board of Education to highlight effects of segregation.
Adolescent Brain & Law
Neuroscience research showing immaturity of prefrontal cortex in adolescents influenced U.S. Supreme Court rulings against juvenile death penalty and life without parole.
Nativism
View that certain knowledge/abilities (e.g., language, number sense, object permanence) are innate and hardwired into humans at birth.
Empiricism
Belief that infants are a "blank slate" (tabula rasa) and knowledge develops entirely through experience and learning
Constructivism
Theory that children are active learners who build knowledge by interacting with the world (e.g., Piaget's stages, Vygotsky's social learning).
Assimilation (Piaget)
Fitting new experiences into existing schemas (e.g., calling a zebra a "horse").
Accommodation (Piaget)
Changing schemas when new information doesn't fit (e.g., learning that a zebra is a new category)
Zone of Proximal Development (Vygotsky)
The gap between what a child can do independently and what they can do with support ("scaffolding") from a more knowledgeable other.
Reliability
The consistency of a measure (same results across time, observers, or items).
Validity
Whether a test/measure actually assesses what it claims to (accuracy).
Violation-of-Expectation (VoE)
Infant method where longer looking times to "impossible" events suggest surprise and mental representations. Example: Baillargeon's object permanence studies.
Habituation
Repeated exposure reduces interest; recovery of attention indicates noticing something new
Longitudinal Study
Following the same children over time to track developmental change
Cross-Sectional Study
Comparing children of different ages at one time point to infer developmental trends
Attachment (Bowlby)
Deep emotional bond between infant and caregiver, evolved to ensure survival. Creates "internal working models" for future relationships.
Strange Situation (Ainsworth)
Lab procedure to assess attachment styles by observing infant behavior when caregiver leaves/returns. Results in four categories: Secure, Avoidant, Ambivalent/Resistant, Disorganized
Secure Attachment
Infant explores freely with caregiver as secure base, upset when caregiver leaves, comforted when they return
Avoidant Attachment
Infant avoids/ignores caregiver, shows little distress upon separation, avoids comfort on return
Ambivalent/Resistant Attachment
Infant is clingy, very distressed when caregiver leaves, but ambivalent (seeks comfort yet resists) upon return
Disorganized Attachment
Confused, inconsistent behavior (e.g., approaching caregiver but looking fearful); often linked to neglect or trauma
Temperament
Early-appearing, biologically based differences in reactivity and regulation
Thomas & Chess
Identified "easy," "difficult," and "slow-to-warm-up" temperament styles.
Kagan
Focused on inhibition vs. uninhibited temperaments (shy vs. bold children).
Rothbart
Defined temperament by reactivity (how easily a child reacts) and self-regulation (how well they control responses)
Differential Susceptibility (Orchid vs. Dandelion)
Some children (orchids) are highly sensitive to environment (do very well in supportive homes, poorly in adverse ones), while others (dandelions) are resilient regardless of conditions
Basic Emotions Theory (Ekman)
Emotions (e.g., happiness, anger, fear, disgust, surprise, sadness) are universal, biologically hardwired, and expressed similarly across cultures.
Psychological Constructionism (Barrett)
Emotions are not innate categories but are constructed from core affect (valence + arousal) combined with learned concepts.
Development of Emotion
3-4 months: distinguish pleasant vs. unpleasant. 4-6 months: integrate multiple cues (face + voice). 5-7 months: distinguish smiles vs. scowls. 2-3 years: match words to faces. 10-12 years: understand mixed emotions
Empathy (Affective)
Affective: feeling with another (crying when another baby cries)
Empathy (Cognitive)
Cognitive: understanding another's perspective or emotions (recognizing a peer is sad)
Hoffman's Stages of Empathy
Global empathy (infants), egocentric empathy (toddlers), recognizing others' feelings, empathy for broader life conditions (older children).
Eisenberg's Stages of Prosocial Reasoning
From self-focused ("helping makes me feel good") to needs-oriented, approval-driven, role-taking, and finally internalized values
Davidov et al. (2020)
Found even infants show concern, inquiry, and prosocial behaviors in response to others' distress.
Theory of Mind
The ability to attribute mental states (beliefs, desires, knowledge) to oneself and others, recognizing they can differ from reality
False Belief Task (Sally-Anne, Wimmer & Perner, 1983)
Child predicts where Sally will look for her marble. 3-year-olds fail (say reality), 4-5-year-olds succeed (say false belief).
Unexpected Contents Task
A crayon box full of candles: Can children say what they thought was inside and what others will think?
Appearance-Reality Task
Sponge that looks like a rock: Do children distinguish appearance from underlying reality?
Advanced ToM (AToM)
Goes beyond first-order false belief. Includes second-order ("He thinks that she thinks..."), irony, sarcasm, lies, faux pas, and interpreting ambiguous info.
9-Month Revolution
Infants begin to understand intentionality and goal-directed action around 9 months (e.g., gaze following, pointing)
Sticky Mittens Experiment (Sommerville)
Infants given mittens to grab objects showed improved recognition of others' goal-directed actions.
Onishi & Baillargeon (2005)
15-month-olds looked longer when an actor searched incorrectly for an object, suggesting some false-belief sensitivity.
Nativitists
infants truly represent beliefs
Constructivists
infants only track perception/last-seen locations
Language & EF Hypothesis
3-year-olds fail verbal false-belief tasks due to underdeveloped language or executive function, not lack of ToM.
Psychological Essentialism
Belief that categories (animals, people, social groups) have an invisible, unchanging "essence" that defines them
Natural Kinds vs. Artifacts
Children treat natural categories (e.g., animals, plants) as biologically determined and unchangeable, but artifacts (e.g., tools, toys) as defined by human intention
Measures of Essentialist Thinking
(1) Immutability (can a raccoon become a skunk?)
(2) Underlying reality (looks like Y, but is X inside)
(3) Nature vs. nurture (raised by monkeys—eat carrots or bananas?)
(4) Category boundaries (rigid vs. flexible).
Rhodes et al. (2014)
Found children are more essentialist about animal categories than artifact categories
Generic Language Effect (Zarpie study)
Using generics ("Zarpies eat flowers") makes children believe categories have stronger, more stable traits, encouraging essentialist thinking.
Social Essentialism
Applied to groups like race, gender, or class → contributes to stereotyping, prejudice, and rigid group boundaries
Application of Clark Doll Test
Influenced Brown v. Board of Education by showing effects of segregation on children's psychology
Why Study Child Development?
Helps improve education, inform parenting, guide social policy, and understand developmental disorders
Example of Assimilation
A child calls a zebra a "horse" because it fits their schema for four-legged animals
Example of Accommodation
A child learns zebras are a new category, adjusting their schema beyond "horse."
Piaget vs. Vygotsky
Piaget: child-driven, universal stages.
Vygotsky: socially driven, no fixed stages, culture matters
Core Knowledge Theories
Propose that infants are born with domain-specific modules (e.g., number, physics, language)
Theory-Theory
Children form and revise "mini theories" about the world, similar to little scientists
Statistical Learning
Infants track probabilities in their environment (e.g., syllable patterns in speech) to learn structure
Reliability Example
If two observers code the same infant behavior and agree → high inter-rater reliability
Validity Example
A vocabulary test is valid if it truly measures word knowledge, not just memory.
Limitation of Correlational Studies
Cannot infer causation (third-variable problem)
Infant Reaching Method
Tracking where infants reach to infer expectations
Infant Sucking Method
Stronger sucking = attention/interest; weaker = habituation.
Paulus Critique of VoE
Argues infants' longer looking may reflect perceptual novelty, not deep reasoning
Stahl & Kibbe Defense of VoE
Counterargument that despite limitations, VoE remains a powerful tool for testing infant cognition.
Secure Attachment Example
Baby cries when mom leaves, calms when she returns.
Avoidant Attachment Example
Baby shows little emotion when mom leaves or returns
Ambivalent/Resistant Example
Baby is clingy, then angry when caregiver returns
Disorganized Example
Baby freezes, shows fear, or contradictory behaviors toward caregiver
Bowlby's Internal Working Models
Early bonds form mental templates for later relationships
Difficult Temperament
Intense reactions, irregular routines, slow to adapt
Slow-to-Warm-Up Temperament
Low activity, withdraws at first but adapts gradually
Inhibited (Kagan)
Shy, fearful, cautious around new people or environments
Uninhibited (Kagan)
Bold, sociable, enjoys novelty
Ekman's 6 Basic Emotions
Happiness, Sadness, Anger, Fear, Surprise, Disgust
Differential Emotions Theory (DET - Izard)
Emotions emerge in a biologically determined sequence (e.g., distress → anger → self-conscious emotions)
Core Affect
The combination of valence (pleasant vs. unpleasant) and arousal (high vs. low) in psychological constructionism
Mixed Emotions Example
Feeling both happy and sad when a friend moves away.
Affective Empathy Example
Baby cries when hearing another baby cry
Cognitive Empathy Example
Understanding a peer is sad about losing a toy, even if you don't feel sad
Unexpected Contents Example
A crayon box with candles → child fails if they say they always knew it had candles.
Appearance-Reality Example
A sponge painted like a rock → fail if child says it's "really a rock."
Second-Order False Belief
"He thinks that she thinks..." (more complex recursive reasoning)
Strange Stories Task (Happé)
Children tested on irony, sarcasm, or lying
Faux Pas test (Baron-Cohen et al. 1999)
Measures ability to detect socially inappropriate statements.
9-Month Gaze Following
Infants start following adults' eyes, showing sensitivity to others' attention
Goal-Directed Action Study
Infants expect agents to pursue goals (e.g., reaching for same toy consistently)
Criticism of Onishi & Baillargeon
Infants may just be tracking objects/locations, not beliefs
Language Hypothesis
Suggests passing false-belief tasks depends on acquiring complement clauses ("She thinks the ball is in the box")
Executive Function Hypothesis
Failure at age 3 may reflect inhibitory control limits, not lack of ToM.
Immutability Example
"Can a raccoon painted like a skunk become a skunk?" → children usually say no
Underlying Reality Example
"It looks like a rock, but it's really a sponge."
Nature vs. Nurture Example
A boy raised by monkeys: does he eat bananas or normal human food?
Category Boundary Beliefs
Children often think animal categories are rigid (a dog is always a dog).
Generic Language Example
Saying "Zarpies eat flowers" encourages kids to think all Zarpies share this trait
Essentialism & Social Groups
Leads to stereotyping (e.g., "boys are naturally good at math")
Cultural Transmission of Essentialism
Parents, teachers, and media pass essentialist beliefs through language and norms
What is the difference between Piaget's stage theory and Vygotsky's sociocultural theory?
Piaget emphasized universal stages of cognitive development; Vygotsky stressed the role of culture and social interaction in shaping learning
What does "domain-specific" vs "domain-general" mean in theories of child development?
Domain-specific = skills develop separately (e.g., language vs. motor). Domain-general = skills grow from a unified set of processes.
What is a strength of longitudinal research in child psychology?
It tracks the same individuals over time, allowing study of developmental trajectories.