1/10
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
background
who was Stanley Milgram?
born in 1933, NYC (Jewish family)
education (PhD from Harvard)
influences: worked with Solomon Asch
academic positions: Yale, Harvard, City university of NY
Motivations
influenced by:
the Holocaust: how could ordinary people commit atrocities
Hannah Arendts ‘Banality of Evil’
desire to make psychological experiments more relevant than line judgements
the studies
study series
24 different versions
over 780 participants
often mistaken as one study
famous ‘basic’ study (no 5)
participant = ‘teacher’, confederate = ‘learner’
learner mentions heart condition, expressed pain, begs to stop
experimenter gives 4 prods:
please continue
the experiment requires that you continue
it is absolutely essential that you continue
you have no other choice, you must go on
results
65% of participants administered the maximum 450 volts
gradual dropouts from early shock levels, but most continued
methods and ethical issues
ethical concerns
described as ‘vile’ and comparable to Nazi experimenters
physical and emotional distress:
14/40 showed nervous laughter
3/40 had seizures
criticism: participants weren’t truly free to withdraw
debriefing controversies (perry, 2012)
minimal or delayed debriefing (sometimes 1 year later)
only last 4 of 23 studies had proper immediate debriefing
Milgram delayed to avoid participants sharing study details
follow up feedback
feeling | % of participants |
extremely upset | 64% |
somewhat nervous | 29% |
not bothered at all | 10% |
replications and variations
replications (1963-1985)
across the USA, Europe, Middle East, Australia
65% compliance was the modal result
variations (key examples)
variation | % obedient |
basic study | 65 |
less prestigious setting | 48 |
learner in same room | 40 |
learner touched | 30 |
experimenter absent | 22 |
peer rebels | 10 |
public predictions (before study) | 1% guessed obedience |
debate and controversy
methodological criticisms
participants may have suspected deception (recorded sounds, unresponsive learner)
lab assistant noted 50% doubters →only 1/3 truly obedient?
criticism: low ecological validity (unrealistic setup)
real-world parallels
Holfing et al (1996): nurses obeyed dangerous doctor orders
Sheridan and King (1972): puppy shows; real victims
Slater et al (2006): virtual victims
theoretical explanations
milgrams interpretation
agentic state: participant transfers responsibility to authority
incremental commitment: foot-in-the-door escalation
self consistency: once they start, harder to stop
social impact theory (Latane, 1981)
obedience = f(strength x immediacy x number)
self categorisation theory (turner, 1999)
obedience higher when:
identification with the authority/scientist is high
identification with the victim is low
know as engaged follower-ship (Haslam and Reicher)
Burger (2009)
prod | obey (%) | disobey (%) |
‘please continue’ | 64 | 36 |
‘experiment requires’ | 46 | 54 |
‘it is essential’ | 10 | 90 |
‘you must go on’ | 0 | 100 |
shows obedience is automatic; resistance increases with perceived coercion
impact and legacy
scientific and cultural influence
most famous social psychology experiment
impacted:
ethics in research
understanding of tyranny, genocide, compliance, management
debate across disciplines: theology, law, history, sociology
mixed academic reception
Wrightsman (1974): criticised lack of theory and rigour
still remains iconic for revealing obedience in ordinary people
core takeaway
Milgrams studies revealed that ordinary people, under certain conditions, may engage in destructive obedience, not from cruelty, but from a sense of obligation and identification with authority