1/221
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
New Englishes: Intro
3rd of 4 Diasporas, so Englishes established during colonial era, also called outer circle variants
substantial influence from indigenous languages → extreme differences between these variations
common similarities, dental fricatives are missing
Singapore Eng: speaker profile
49 years old, ethnically Chinese, Singapore-born
English is the main language used at home and work
First language Peranakan Patois (Malay) and Hokkien
Singapore Eng: Phonological features
Centralized vowels
Little or no vowel reduction (unstressed vowels stay full)
Syllable-based rhythm (equal timing per syllable)
/th/ → [t] in „thought“, „threaten“, „third”
/th/ → [d] in „there“ and „this“
Final /t/ often omitted
Simplification of word-final consonant clusters very frequent
Use of full vowels in function words like as, of, for
Unstressed syllables of polysyllabic words
Stressing of pronouns
Nigerian Eng: speaker profile
51 years old
First language Idoma used at home, also speaks Hausa
Speaks English with his friends, colleagues, everyone outside his family
Nigerian Eng: Phonological features
Full vowels in unstressed syllables of polysyllabic words
Syllable-based rhythm
/th/ → [d] in „this“ and some tokens of „the“
[g] at the end of words such as sing, hang, long
LOT in „cousins“ and „company“
Use of full vowels in function words like as, of, for
Substantial variation in accent throughout the country
Avoidance of dental fricatives
Omission of plosives from word-final consonant clusters
Indian Eng: speaker profile
45 years old, from the south of India
First language Malayalam used with family members, also speaks some Hindi
Uses English with friends and colleagues
Indian Eng: phonological features
Centralized vowel space
Minimal vowel reduction
[t] in thought, threaten, third
[d] in this and than
Retroflex quality for [t] and [d]
Sporadic merger of [v] and [w]
[j] or [w] before words that start with a vowel (did not occur in this case)
Syllable-based rhythm
reduced vowels
outer circle Englishes frequently use vowels even in unstressed syllables and function words
rhythm
due to absence of reduced vowels its perceived as syllabic rhythm in many outer circle variants
syllabic rhythm is very common in outer circle englishes
New Zealand Eng (NZE): historical background
inner circle Eng, mainly British immigrants
treaty of Waitangi (1840)
dialect formation (1870-1890)
education act (1877): unified schooling
trudgills dialect formation theory: accomodation → variation → focusing
NZE: Phonology
consonants:
non-rhotic
linking /r/
flapped /t/
TH-fronting
full /h/
vowels:
diphthong shifts: PRICE → [ɑə], FACE → [ae], MOUTH → [æə]
KIT vowel distinctive → [ɘ] or [ə]
NZE: syntax & lexis
syntax: british-based
lexis:
Mix of UK/US terms
Maori loanwords (e.g. whānau, tangi) – no plural -s
- “eh?” as tag
High Rising Terminal (uptalk)
Canadian Eng: historical background
Part of Third Dialect Area (NE USA, Western PA, Western States)
Believed to be uniform, but has distinct vowel features
Sociolinguistic change linked to age and region
CanE: Phonology
Canadian Shift: /ɪ/ → /ɛ/ → /æ/ lowering and retraction
Canadian Raising:
/aɪ/, /aʊ/ raised before voiceless codas (e.g. house vs. houses)
Regional variation (6 regions)
Fronting of /ɑr/ (e.g. start)
CanE: lexis & american influence
Indigenous loanwords (e.g. kayak, muskeg)
Americanization via media, but with resistance
Examples:
leisure with/without /r/
news → [nuːz]
Tuesday → [tuːzdeɪ]
Australian Eng: historical background
Introduced in 1770 (James Cook)
Koinéization of dialects from SE England, Ireland, Scotland
Population largely convicts or ex-convicts
AusE: Phonology
Consonants:
Non-rhotic with intrusive/linking /r/
/l/ vocalization (e.g. milk → [mɪuk])
Syllabic nasals and laterals
H-deletion in unstressed words (e.g. him → /im/)
Vowels:
Rounded /ɜ/ (e.g. bird)
Monophthongal [ɔ:] in poor, sure
Monophthong variants for diphthongs (near, square)
AusE: grammar
Pronouns:
Gender on inanimate objects (e.g. I put her [beer] down)
Use of me instead of I
Non-standard variants
Verbs:
Present perfect preferred over simple past
AusE: prosodic features: high rising tone on declarative clauses
Common in narratives and descriptions
Occurs in construction of extended turns
Phonetics teaching focus
helps students produce & recognize individual sounds clearly
phonology teaching focus
help students sound natural, understanding rhythm & patterns
segmental features: vowels & consonants (common difficulties)
confusing vowel length & quality → /iː/ (sheep) vs. /ɪ/ (ship)
difficulty producing dental fricatives → /θ/ and /ð/ (often replaced by /s/ or /d/)
trouble distinguishing minimal pairs → (bit vs. beat, thin vs. tin)
segmental features: vowels & consonants (teaching tools)
IPA charts: visual mapping of English sounds → recognition & production
minimal pairs practice: drills to improve discrimination & production accuracy
tongue twisters: fun, repetitive practice for muscle memory
phoneme bingo: gamified learning of individual sounds
receptive before productive: train students to hear distinctions before producing
focus on intelligibility, not native-like perfection
suprasegmental features: word stress
unpredictable & can change meaning
stress placement critical
techniques:
dictionaries that mark stress
physical activities → clapping, tapping syllables
visual aids: underlining stressed syllables
emphasize stress in new vocabulary teaching
supresegmental features: sentence stess & prominence
content words usually stressed
function words often unstressed or weak
teaching:
marking stress on scripts
explaining tone units & tonic syllables
intonation patterns influence meaning → emphasis can change sentence interpretation
connected speech & schwa
weak forms: unstressed words pronounced less clearly
schwa: most common vowel, appears in unstressed syllables
elision: omission of sounds
assimilation: sound change to become more like neighboring sounds
teaching:
improves listening comprehension of fluent speech
helps learners speak more naturally and fluently
de-schwaed texts: fill in missing schwa sounds
stress only reading: say only stressed syllables to feel rhythm
sentence recognition: write out citation forms from connected speech samples
pronunciation in practice
modeling in context:
present new words and phrases in meaningful sentences
use repetition, recording, encourage self-correction
chants & rhythm activities:
use poems, dialogues, chants focusing on rhythm and weak forms
encourage group participation and memory retention
shadow reading:
students read aloud simultaneously with a recording
builds rhythm, pitch variation, chunking of speech
best with short, repeated texts
voice settings:
teach students the “feel” of english speech
pitch, range, relaxed mouth articulation, mouth shape & movement
include practice with nonsense words for fun and focus on articulation
RP vs. ELF: Which Model to teach?
RP:
traditional “standard” accent in Eng teaching
spoken by less than 3% of UK population
seen as elitist, outdated by many
not globally representative
ELF:
reflects Eng use between non-native speakers worldwide
prioritize intelligbility, not accent imitation
allows accent variation & inclusiveness
encourages learners to develop their own clear Eng voice
teaching implications:
use RP as a reference, not strict standard
expose students to multiple Eng varieties
focus on clear, confident communication
intonation
also called pitch contour or pitch movement
all languages have intonation
intonation is principally the variation of pitch, but also prominence over a stretch of speech
structural function
signals the grammatical or structural role of an utterance
determines whether
question
request
instruction
accentual function
affects prominence of a syllable
plays a role in focusing stress on particular words in connected speech
attitudinal function
conveys the speaker’s personal orientations towards what they say, or gives us clues about how speakers feel (uninterested, excited, ironic...)
discourse function
marks the turntaking processes in an exchange between speakers
pitch
frequency of the vibration of the vocal folds
fundamental frequency of the voice
the faster vocal folds vibrate the higher the pitch
fundamental frequencies of individual speakers are not significant in terms of segmental phonology
all speakers can use intonation to achieve the functions → every speaker controls their pitch
tone unit
we can break connected speech down into utterances (units that begin & end with a clear pause)
within an utterance we cann identify smaller units over which a single intonation contour extends → tone unit
connected speech consists of..
utterances ← one or more tone units ← one or more feet ← one or more syllables ← one or more phonemes
intonation patterns
in a tone unit one or more syllables are usually more prominent than others
last prominent syllable is called tonic syllable / nucleus → main pitch movement begins
carries tonic stress / nucleus stress → determines particular intonation pattern
intonation paterrn: fall
WHERE do you (H) LIVE (TS) (neutral question)
most neutral tone in RP
signals finality & definiteness → marking end of thought or sentence
commonly used in wh-questions
→ There were three people there.
→ How are you?
→ That’s all I have to say.
intonation pattern: rise
WHERE have you (H) BEEN (TS) (angry parent to a child)
primarily in yes/no questions
common in requests for repetition / clarification
indicates dominant / authorative tone → commands, instructions, aggressive questioning
used when listing except for the last one
→ Do you live near hear?
→ apples, oranges, bananas and pears.
→ First turn left, then turn right.
intonation pattern: fall-rise
I’LL (PH) BE there (H) SOON (TS) (reassurance)
express equal participation in discourse
confirming shared information, asking for permission, offering reassurance
feeling of cooperation & understanding between speaker & listener
→ We’re leaving at seven, aren’t we?
→ It’ll be alright.
→ May I open the window?
intonation pattern: rise-fall
the (PH) FILM was (H) WON (TS) derful (T) (emphatic statement)
strong personal impression → show surprise, excitement, enthusiasm
strong positive attitudes towards sth
→ That’s a lovely view!
→ What a goal!
intonation pattern: high key
HOW much did you (H) PAY (TS) (question signalling surprise at the price)
indicates surprise, strong disagreement, strong agreement
tone higher than usual → emphasize emotions or highlight a statement
→ Only 28? I thought you were at least 35!
→ Actually I think you’re wrong!
→ I quite agree!
tone unit structure
4 components:
pre-head (PH): spans all the less prominent syllables before the head
Head (H): part of a tone unit which extends from the first prominent syllable up to but not including tonic syllable
tonic syllable (TS): last prominent syllable → part where the main pitch movement begins
tail (T): comprises all syllables that occur between the tonic syllable at the end of the tone unit
allophone
one of two or more pronunciation variants of a phoneme
→ phonetically different but represent the same phoneme and do not change the meaning of a word
2 key criteria for identifying allophones
no minimal pair (no word pair distinguished by these sounds)
articulatory similartiy between the variants
[t] – clear stop: [ˈbʌtər] (formal speech)
[ɾ] – flap: [ˈbʌɾər] (American English)
[ʔ] – glottal stop: [ˈbʌʔə] (British English)
→ All versions are allophones of /t/, and the meaning of butter remains the same
types of allophony: free variation
allophones appear in same phonetic environment
no fixed rule → choice depends on speaker, accent, style
→ butter may be pronounced [ˈbʌtər], [ˈbʌɾər], or [ˈbʌʔə].
types of allophony: complementary distribution
allophones occur in different environments
rule that determines which allophone appears where
top vs stop
→ [th] in top: aspirated /t/ at the beginning of a stressed syllable
→ [t] in stop: unaspirated /t/ after /s/
phonological proccesses that create allophones: aspiration
Voiceless stops /p/, /t/, /k/ are aspirated before stressed vowels
kill → [khɪl], skill → [skɪl]
phonological processes that create allophones: devoicing
Voiced sounds lose voicing after voiceless stops.
/l/ becomes [l̥ ] in please, clean
phonological processes that create allophones: syllabic consonants
A consonant serves as the syllable nucleus.
button → [ˈbʌtn̩ ], little → [ˈlɪtl̩ ]
phonological processes that create allophones: elision & linking
Elision: deletion of a sound in connected speech
next day → [nɛks deɪ]
Linking: insertion of a sound for smoother transition
law and order → [lɔːr ən ɔːdə]
articulation definition
how humans produce speech sounds via the movement of vocal organs
involves coordinated effort of various parts of the vocal tract (lips, teeth, tongue, alveolar ridge, hard palate, soft palate (velum), glottis
→ /p/ = made with lips (bilabial)
→ /k/ = made with back of your tongue and soft palate (velar)
secondary articulation
speech sound is made with a main place (primary articulation) and an extra, less important one at the same time
Example: sounds with /w/ (water)
Primary articulation = velar (made with the back of the tongue near the soft palate)
Secondary articulation = labialized (rounding of lips at the same time)
→ /w/=labialized velar sound
anytime two articulations are used together → Corarticulation
types of secondary articulation: Labialisation
The rounding of the lips while making a sound elsewhere in the mouth
→ /w/ is a labialised velar: tongue at velum and lips round
→ Happens when a sound picks up a “w”-like quality
types of secondary articulation: Palatalisation
The front of the tongue is raised to the hard palate while making another sound
→ Think of how “cute” sounds vs. “coot”, the [k] in cute is palatalised
→ Adds a “y” quality, almost like putting a hint of /j/ into the sound
→ In Russian: /tj/, /nj/, etc. – the j = palatalisation
types of secondary articulation: velarisation
Making a normal sound like /l/, but the back of the tongue pulls back toward the velum (soft palate)
→ In English “dark l” in “feel” or “milk”. - (“Dark L” [ɫ] – a velarised alveolar lateral
(alveolar is primary, back of tongue raised is secondary)
→ Makes the sound “darker” or “heavier”. Can be felt how the tongue is pushing back
types of secondary articulation: phryngealization
Space is narrowed in the throat while making another sound
→ Found in Arabic a lot (like the emphatic /sʕ/)
→ Makes the sound more “guttural” or throaty. It’s tensing the root of the tongue toward the throat
nasalisation
air escapes through the nose as well as the mouth when making a sound → because velum is lowered letting air flow into the nose
Example: the word “man” said slowly
/m/ = nasal consonant (always nasal)
But also, vowel /æ/ in “man” is affected and becomes nasalised: [mæ̃n]
→ not secondary articulation
general infomation
part of the field of phonology & phonetics
deals with pronunciation & sound systems of languages
enhances spoken fluency
makes learners sound more natural & native-like
strong form
pronunciation vatiant of words with strong vowels + no sound that has been omitted
occur in prominent & non-prominent positions → stressed vs. unstressed
weak form
pronunciation variant of words with weak vowels / > 1 sound has been omitted
only occur in non-prominent positions → always unstressed
grammatical words (function words)
words with primarily grammatical function but no lexical content
pronouns
prepositions
auxiliaries
conjunctions
determiners
→ these word classes are closed (new words are rarely ever added)
grammatical words: determiners
orthography | strong form(s) | weak form(s) |
---|---|---|
the | ðiː | ðɪ, ðə |
a/an | eɪ, an | ə, ən |
some | sʌm | səm, sm̩ |
grammatical words: conjunctions
orthography | strong form(s) | weak form(s) |
---|---|---|
and | and | ənd, ən, n̩d, n̩ |
but | bʌt | bət |
that | ðat | ðət |
grammatical words: pronouns
orthography | strong form(s) | weak form(s) |
---|---|---|
his | hɪz | ɪz |
him | hɪm | ɪm |
her | hɜː | hə, ə, ɜː |
you | juː | jʊ, jə |
your | joː | jə |
she | ʃiː | ʃɪ |
he | hiː | ɪ |
we | wiː | wɪ |
them | ðɛm | ðəm, əm |
us | ʌs | əs, s |
grammatical words: prepositions
orthography | strong form(s) | weak form(s) |
---|---|---|
than | ðan | ðən |
at | at | ət |
for | foː | fə |
from | fɹɒm | fɹəm, fəm, fm̩ |
of | ɒv | əv, v |
to | tuː | tə, tʊ |
as | az | əz, z |
there | ðɛə | ðə |
grammatical words: auxiliaries
orthography | strong form(s) | weak form(s) |
---|---|---|
can | kan | kən, kn̩ |
could | kʊd | kəd |
have | hav | əv, v |
has | haz | əz, z |
had | had | əd, d |
will | wɪl | l |
shall | ʃal | ʃəl, ʃl, l |
should | ʃʊd | ʃəd |
must | mʌst | məs, məst |
do | duː | də, d |
does | dʌz | dəz, z |
am | am | əm, m̩ |
are | ɑː | ə |
was | wɒz | wəz |
were | wɜː | wə |
been | biːn | bɪn |
lexical words (content words)
words with lexical meaning
nouns
adverds
adjectives
full verbs
→ these word classes are open (new words are contineously added)
words are always in their weak form unless they are…
…then they become strong
Weak Strong
Isolated | A: "Who did it?" B: "You did." | /jə/ or /ju/ | /juː/ |
Quoted | A: "Did he say 'can' or 'can't'?" B: "He definitely said 'can'." | /kən/ | /kæn/ |
At the end of a phrase or sentence | “Who did you give it to?” | /tə/ /frəm/ | /tuː/ /frɒm/ |
As the first of 2 consecutive auxiliary verbs without a full verb | A: "He said he would help." B: "Well, he should have!" | /ʃəd/ | /ʃʊd/ |
In coordination | “He travels to and from London.” "I said to John and to Mary." | /tə/ /frəm/ | /tuː/ /frɒm/ |
In contrast | "They said they would come, but they didn’t." | (usually only strong form) | /ˈdɪd(ə)nt/ |
Used to emphasise a particular aspect of the message | "I do like your new haircut!" | /də/ or /du/ | /duː/ |
steps to transcription
look at word class
grammatical word → weak
lexical word → strong or weak
if one of the rules applies they are strong
pay attention to special cases (exceptions)
special cases
Strong and weak forms of some grammatical words end in a linking sound:
a) a tree → before consonant
b) an apple → before vowel
Auxiliary verbs + not (negation) → strong form:
I can speak German. vs. I can’t speak German.
Do, the and to have 2 weak forms:
a) before consonant → the door /ə/
b) before vowel → the end \[i], \[u]
Had, has, have, he and her drop their initial /h/:
I like his car. /ɪz/ (except at the beginning of the sentence)
Some:
a) There are some children waiting. → as determiner → weak
b) Some like it cold.* → as pronoun → strong
There:
a) There’s no place like home! → existential → weak
b) Look over there→ adverb of place → strong
Liaison (French Link)
process of joining sounds from adjacent words in spoken language
linking sound
a sound that is absent in a word when that word is pronounced in isolation, but present in the same word in certain phonetic environments in connected speech
Why is linking important?
it makes speech smoother & more fluent
improves listening comprehension
enhances pronunciation & rhythm
reduces misunderstandings
helps avoid awkward pauses or breaks between words
types of linking: Consonant + vowel linking
word ends in consonant, next word starts with vowel
→ ex. not at all /ˌnɒdəˈtɔːl/
common: /t/ /d/ /n/
types of linking: consonant + consonant linking
same consonant appears at word boundary
only pronounced once, smoothly
→ “big game” /bɪgeɪm/
types of linking: linking r (only in non-rhotic accents)
final “r” pronounced only before vowel
→ “far away” /fɑːr əˈweɪ/
link between word through the articulation of a normally unarticulated word-final /r/
r-sound in question occurs in the spelling in the form of a letter <r>
types of linking: intrusive sounds (/r/ /j/ /w/)
sounds added between vowel-vowel boundaries
not in the spelling
“law and order” → /lɔː r ənd ˈɔːdə/ (a “r” sound)
“idea of it” → /aɪˈdɪər əv ɪt/
“i agree” → /aɪ jəˈgriː/ → y sound
“go on” → /gəʊ ˈwɒn/ → w sound
intrusive r is very common in non-rhotic accents
helps avoid hiatus (vowel clash)
accent variation: rhotic
r-ful accent
the /r/ phoneme is articulated wherever it occurs in the spelling
accent variation: non-rhotic
the /r/ phoneme is articulated only before a vowel not before a consonant or pause
Juncture (boundary signal): pause (speech pauses)
can be silent or filled with hesitation sounds like “uh”
less frequent in natural speech
Juncture (boundary signals): phonotactic rules
rules about which sounds (phonemes) are allowed in certain positions within a word
Juncture (boundary signals): prosodic features
include stress, intonation, and length of sounds
can signal the start of a new word or phrase
Juncture (boundary signals): articulatory features (phonetic transitions)
subtle changes in how sounds are produced at word boundaries
rhythm
stress timing & syllable compression
stressed syllables occur at roughly equal time intervals
unstressed syllables are shortened to fit the rhythm
function words are often reduced
I want to go → /aɪ ˈwɒnə ˈgəʊ/
he could have done it →/hi kʊdə dʌn ɪt/
weak forms commonly use schwa sound in unstressed positions
creates melodic, bouncy flow in Eng (stress-timed language)
Assimilation
process where sounds become similar or identical to a neighbouring sound (typically a word boundary)
may involve:
place of articulation changes (alveolar → bilabial)
voicing changes (voiceless → voiced)
manner of articulation adaptation (stop → fricative)
types of assimilation: progressive
a sound is influenced by the one before it
→ dogs /dɒɡz/ (voiced /g/ affects /s/)
types of assimilation: regressive
a sound is influenced by the one that follows
/ɡʊd bɔɪ/ → (voiced bilabial /b/ influences /d/
types of assimilation: reciprocal
two sounds influence each other
“dont you” → /dəʊntʃu/
elision
omission of a sound for ease of pronunciation in rapid or casual speech → it often affects consonant clusters or unstressed vowels
more likely:
in clusters involving plosives (/t/ /d/)
in casual or fast speech
when articulatory effort is reduced to maintain fluency
common elision patterns: consonant elision
friendship → /ˈfrɛnʃɪp/ (the /d/ is dropped)
common elision patterns: cluster simplification
next day → /nek̬s deɪ/ (the /t/ is dropped)
common elision patterns: vowel elision
family → /ˈfæmli/ (middle vowel is omitted)
intro to German language
indo-european language
variety of dialects
similarities in phonology, vocabulary, syntax
when language has restrictions on phonotactics it applies to every word (also non-native words)
problem: languages seek to overcome problems of borrowing a foreign word that violates their phonotactics
solution: it varies from language and creates accents
english example: problem: two stops cannot come at the beginning of words; also cannot stop + nasal combination
words like ptolemy & gnostic will be pronounced differently by English speakers
will drop the first two consonants → [tɑləmi] & [nɑstɪk]
worlds like Gdansk & knish will be pronounced differently by English speakers
will insert a vowel between the two consonants → [gədænsk] & [kənɪʃ]
Finnish: avoids syllables containing sequences of consonants
solution: tries to fix borrowed words with consonant clusters by trying to “repair” it
deletion
in loan word first of a series of consonants get dropped (if they do not conform to its phonotactics)
(CCCVNC) → (CVNCV)
→ addition of a final vowel to avoid consonant in the syllable-final position
Japanese: avoids syllables containing sequences of consonants
solution: insertion
rule-governed (always works the same way)
→ easy to predict shape & pronunciation of loan words
the vowel /u/ is inserted, except after /t/ and /d/, where /o/ will be inserted
→ /bɑːθ/ → /basu/
/kəntroʊl/ → /kontoroːru/
→ /u/ inserted in both words to keep the word-final syllables from ending in a consonant & second [o] inserted to prevent [t] & [r] from forming a cluster
sound substitution
a process whereby sounds that do not exist in the language when borrowing or trying to pronounce a foreign word
→ a few languages have fewer or more phonemes or allophones than English does → audible when non-native speakers of Eng pronounce Eng
substitutions by non-native speakers and strategies for handling phonotactic both result in forein accents + changes in words that have been borrowed into another language
French speaker pronouncing Eng:
this [ðɪs] as [zɪs]
thin [θɪn] as [sɪn]
→ french phonemic inventory doesn’t contain ð/ or /θ/ so french speakers substitute with nearest equivalent sound the fricatives /z/ and /s/
German vs. Eng: Phonology
broadly similar
german accent more energetic articulation
tenser vowels
more explosive stop consonants (/p/, /t/, /k/)
more lip rounding/ spreading
general lower or higher pitch
frequent use of glottal stops before initial vowels