1/101
Looks like no tags are added yet.
Name | Mastery | Learn | Test | Matching | Spaced |
---|
No study sessions yet.
what does aggression mean
refers to a range of behaviors that can result in both physical and psychological harm to oneself, other people, or objects in the environment
outline neural influences on aggression: the limbic system
area of the brain, lying beneath the cortex, that helps to coordinate behaviour that satisfy motivational and emotional urges, such as aggression and fear
contains amygdala and hippocampus
outline neural influences on aggression: amygdala
responsible for quickly evaluating the emotional importance of sensory info and promoting an appropriate response
if certain areas of the amygdala are stimulated electrically, an animal responds with aggression e.g. snarling
kluver and bucy found that the destruction of the amygdala in a monkey who was dominant in a social group caused it to lose its dominant place as no longer responded the same way
outline neural influences on aggression: hippocampus
involved in the formation of long term memories so allows an animal to compare the conditions of a current threat with similar past experiences and respond in a way that increases chances of survival
impaired hippocampal function prevents the nervous system from putting things into a relevant and meaningful context so may cause the amygdala to respond inappropriately, resulting in aggressive behaviour
boccardi et al. found that habitually violent offenders exhibited abnormalities of hippocampal functioning
outline neural influences on aggression: effect of serotonin on the amygdala
low levels of serotonin remove the inhibitory effect on the amygdala, with the consequence that individuals are less able to control impulsive and aggressive behaviour (‘serotonin deficiency hypothesis’)
so when the amygdala was stimulated it becomes more active causing the person to act on their impulses and making aggression more likely
outline neural influences on aggression: influence of serotonin
low levels of serotonin in the brain have associated with an increased susceptibility to impulsive behaviour and aggression as fails to inhibit response to emotional stimuli
some drugs are though to alter serotonin and increase aggressive behaviour
mann et al. gave 35 healthy participants ‘deplete serotonin’, using a questionnaire to assess hostility and aggression levels, found that males were associated with an increase in hostility and aggression scores
what is testosterone
hormone produced mainly be the testes associated with the development of secondary sex characteristics in males and implicated in aggressive behaviour
outline hormonal influences on aggression: effect of testosterone on the brain
influence aggression from young adulthood onwards due to its action on brain areas involved in controlling aggression
sapolsky summarised research evidence in this area by describing how removing the source of testosterone in different specifics typically resulted in much lower levels of aggression
changes in testosterone levels appear to influence aggressive behaviour by increasing amygdala reactivity during the processing of a social threat
outline hormonal influences on aggression: gender differences in the effect of testosterone
archer found men are generally more aggressive than women
dabbs found men have much higher concentration of testosterone than women
daly and wilson found that when testosterone concentrations are at their highest (21 to 35) there is an increase in male-on-male aggressive behaviour
what is meant by genetic factors in terms of aggression
likelihood of behaving aggressively is determined by an individual’s genetic makeup that is inherited from their parents
outline the role of genetic factors in aggression: twin studies
MZ twins share all of their genes whereas DZ twins share only 50%
coccaro et al. studied adult twin pairs found nearly 50% if the variance in direct aggressive behaviour could be attributed to genetic factors
outline the role of genetic factors in aggression: adoption studies
hutchings and mednick studied 14,000 adoptions, finding that a significant number of adopted boys with criminal convictions had biological parents particularly fathers, with convictions for criminal violence
providing evidence for genetic effect as suggests that aggression is a trait that can be inherited from parent to offspring
outline the role of genetic factors in aggression: research from miles and carey
carried out a meta-analysis of 24 twin and adoption studies, showing the genetic basis of aggression
found a strong genetic influence could account for as much as 50% of the variance in aggression
found age differences important as both genes and environment are influential in aggression in youth but at later ages the influence of environment decreased as influence of genes increased
outline the role of genetic factors in aggression: research from rhee and waldman
combined the results of 51 twin and adoption studies
concluded aggressive anti-social behaviour was largely a product of genetic contributions
what is MAOA
an enzyme that regulates the metabolism of serotonin in the brain and low levels of serotonin are associated with impulsive and aggressive behaviour
outline the role of genetic factors in aggression: research into the role of MAOA
brunner et al. studied a dutch family, finding that many of its male members behaved in a violent and aggressive manner and a large proportion had been involved in serious crimes of violence
men were found to have abnormally low levels of MAOA in their bodies and a defect in this gene was later identified
outline the role of genetic factors in aggression: variants in MAOA
caspi et al discovered a variant of the gene associated with high levels of MAOA (MAOA-H) and variant associated with low levels (MAOA-L)
found those with MAOA-L were significantly more likely to grow up to exhibit anti-social behaviour but only if they had been maltreated as children
mcdermott et al. found that MAOA-L participants displayed higher levels of aggression when provoked compared to MAOA-H
outline the role of genetic factors in aggression: ‘warrior gene’
MAOA-L is much more frequent in populations with a history of warfare with about ⅔ of people in these populations having this version of the gene
only ⅓ in western populations have those low-activity version of the gene
briefly outline the ethological explanation of aggression
Lorenz stressed the adaptive value of animal behaviours like aggression as it leads to the better use of resources, protection of territories and acquiring reproductive mates
observe aggression in other animals in their natural habitats to understand human aggression
outline the ethological explanation of aggression: fixed action patterns and innate releasing mechanisms
Tinbergen argued that innate behaviours fixed action patterns (FAPs) are a repertoire, that all members of the same species have, of stereotyped behaviour that occurs in specific conditions
when the sign stimulus (specific stimulus) is present it stimulates the sensory recognition circuit
neural network known as innate releasing mechanism (IRM) receives input from the circuit
IRM sends a signal to the motor control circuits to activate the FAPs associated with the sign stimulus
who identified the characteristics of FAPs
Lea
outline the ethological explanation of aggression: characteristics of FAPs
stereotyped - always occurs in the same way
universal - behaviour is the same in all conspecific (i.e. same species)
independent of individual experience - behaviour is innate
ballistic - once triggered, the FAP cannot be changed or stopped
specific triggers - each FAP has a specific sign stimulus
outline the ethological explanation of aggression: tinbergen’s research on FAPs
male stickleback fish will produce a fixed sequence of aggressive actions when another male enters its territory
sign stimulus in this case is not the presence of the other male, but the sight of its distinctive red underbelly acts as a sign stimulus
if this is covered up, the intruder is not attacked
who developed the hydraulic model of instinctive behaviour
Lorenz
outline the ethological explanation of aggression: hydraulic model of instinctive behaviour
each FAP has a reservoir of ‘action-specific energy’ (ASE) that builds up over time placing pressure on IRM
appropriate sign stimulus causes the IRM to release this energy and the animal then performs the FAP
after performing the FAP, the reservoir of ASE is empty and behaviour cannot be repeated until the ASE has built up again
outline the ethological explanation of aggression: ritualistic aggression in animals
not all aggressive behaviour involves fighting but may be ritualised in the form of threat displays e.g. chest pounding
important because they help animals to assess their relative strength before deciding to escalate a conflict
making costly and dangerous physical aggression less likely to occur as opponent may back down
outline the ethological explanation of aggression: ritualistic aggression in humans
evidence of the use of ritualised aggression in tribal warfare in human cultures
gardner and heider described how the dani of new guinea engaged in ritualised patterns of intergroup hostility
outline the ethological explanation of aggression: wolves v. doves theory
Lorenz claimed that species who have ‘evolved weapons’ like strong teeth must also have instinctive inhibitions that prevent them using these against members of their own species
non-hunting species have no such powerful weapons so have not developed the same inhibitions against hurting their own kind
outline the ethological explanation of aggression: wolves v. doves theory in humans
Lorenz believed that humans are more like the dove than the world when it comes to dealing with other human beings
we don't have powerful natural weapons so no need to develop strong instinctive inhibitions against killing one another
briefly outline a strength of the ethological explanation of aggression: research support
Tinbergen found that male stickleback fish will produce a fixed sequence of aggressive actions when the sign stimulus of a red spot is present
briefly outline a criticism of the ethological explanation of aggression: challenging evidence for hydraulic model
Von Holst demonstrated that the aggressive behaviour itself could provide a further stimulus, which, rather than reducing the likelihood of further aggressive behaviour, made it more likely
too simplistic as fails to acknowledge the potential for aggressive behaviour to escalate
briefly outline a criticism of the ethological explanation of aggression: generalisations of instinctive view
Lehrman argued Lorenz underestimated the role of environmental factors in the development of species-typical aggressive behaviour patterns
Eibl-Eibesfeldt suggested that FAPs, such as aggression, are no longer adaptive in modern times due to the environment in which we exist changing rapidly
human behaviour is far more varied and less predictable compared to non-humans due to human beings being more cognitively and socially sophisticated than animals (extrapolation issues with animal studies)
briefly outline a criticism of the ethological explanation of aggression: killing members of the same species isn’t that rare
Goodall found that rival communities slaughtered each other in a systematic fashion, despite appeasement and ritualistic signals being displayed by the victims
suggests some forms of aggression are not innate or adaptive as instinctive inhibitions are not always present
what is meant by evolution
when a species gradually changes over many successive generations
briefly outline the evolutionary explanation of aggression
Duntley and Buss argued adaptations, including those that function to inflict costs on other humans, make up our human nature and are a product of evolution by natural selection
outline the evolutionary explanation of aggression: adaptive behaviour
aggression is strategy that would have been effective for solving a number of adaptive problems among early humans
solving such problems enhance the survival and reproductive success of the individual so the mental module would have spread through the gene pool
mental modules evolved to ‘tell us what to do’ in order to deal with situations similar to those faced by ancestral humans
outline the evolutionary explanation of aggression: sexual competition
ancestral males seeking access to females would have to compete with other males through aggression
those individuals who use aggression successfully against competitors would have been more successful in acquiring mates so would be more successful in passing on their genes to offspring
led to development of a genetically transmitted tendency for males to be aggressive towards other males
outline the evolutionary explanation of aggression: evidence of sexual competition
Puts argues that various male traits seem to imply that competition with other males did take place among ancestral males
e.g. universally males have thicker jawbones, robust skulls, brow ridges and 75% more muscle mass than women (Lassek and Gaulin)
outline the evolutionary explanation of aggression: sexual jealousy
Archer argues aggression can occur as a result of sexual jealousy, which arises as a result of parental uncertainty
men can never be entirely certain that they are fathers of their children, as fertilisation is hidden from them, so are at risk of cuckoldry (reproductive cost as a result of his partners infidelity)
consequence is that a man might unwittingly invest his resources in offspring that are not his own so aggression minimises the risk of infidelity and cuckoldry
outline the evolutionary explanation of aggression: evidence of sexual jealousy
Dobash and Dobash found that the majority of cases women cite extreme jealousy on the part of their partners as the key cause of the violence directed towards them
outline the evolutionary explanation of aggression: consequence of warfare
behaviour associated with warfare would have evolved because of the adaptive benefits for the individual and their offspring e.g. obtain valuable resources, attract mates and forge intragroup bonds
outline the evolutionary explanation of aggression: consequence of warfare on mates
Chagnon found male warriors in traditional societies tend to have more sexual partners and more children, suggesting a direct reproductive benefit
outline the evolutionary explanation of aggression: consequence of warfare on status
aggression in combat can increase status for individual warriors
lead to peers to respect them more as that individual dominates other males
briefly outline a strength of the evolutionary aggression: research support
Buss found that males display a heightened sensitivity to perceived affronts to their status and reputation, such that many acts of male-on-male violence result from one male perceiving a slight to his status from another male
Shackelford et al. found that male retention strategies, such as negative inducements, are a method of expressing sexual jealousy, which leads to aggressive behaviour towards females and other partners to reduce infidelity
briefly outline a criticism of the evolutionary explanation: gender biased
explanation argues women haven’t evolved to be aggressive as did not face reproductive competition and were more likely to select aggressive men to protect their babies (alpha bias)
BUT females also commit aggressive behaviour to express sexual jealousy, but adopt a more social form, by producing behaviours that are designed to harm another person's social status or self-esteem through gossiping etc. (Prinz)
briefly outline a criticism of the evolutionary explanation: cultural differences
explanation would suggest aggression would be uniform across the human species because the motivation for such behaviour is identical (i.e. increase chances of survival and reproduction)
BUT the !Kung San people of the Kalahari have negative attitudes towards the use of aggression so discourage it from childhood as those who use aggression lose their status are diminished
whereas the Yanomamo appear to accept aggression and require the behaviour in order to gain status in their highly structured society
suggests that the innate nature of aggression can be outweighed by non-evolutionary cultural and social factors
who developed the frustration-aggression hypothesis
dollard et al.
briefly outline the frustration-aggression hypothesis
proposed that the existence of frustration (any stimulus preventing an individual from attaining some goal) always leads to some form of aggression
contextual factors, such as the threat of punishment, could inhibit aggressive behaviour in some situations
outline the cause and effect relationship proposed by frustration-aggression hypothesis
external factors prevent goal achievement, resulting in frustration
frustration is increased by strong motivation to achieve a goal, expectation of gratification and lack of control
frustration leads to the arousal of an aggressive drive which leads to aggressive behaviour
aggressive urges can be relieved through the production of an aggressive behaviour which has a cathartic effect on the individual (i.e. an emotional release that is achieved by the person engaging in aggressive behaviour)
outline an example of the frustration-aggression hypothesis: brown et al.
surveyed british holidaymakers who were prevented from travelling by ferry to france because french fishing boats blocked the french port
found an increase in hostile attitudes towards the french as a result of the passengers frustration
outline the frustration-aggression hypothesis: justified and unjustified frustration
doob and sears asked participants to imagine how they would feel in a number of different frustrating situations e.g. waiting for a bus which went by w/o stropping
most participants reported that they would feel angry in all of the frustrating situations
pastore distinguished between justified and unjustified frustration, arguing that it was mainly the unjustified that produces anger and aggression
pastore produced different variations of doob and sears’ research (i.e. justified and unjustified) e.g. added that the bus was displaying a ‘out of service’ message
found under this condition (justified frustration), participants expressed much lower levels of anger
outline the frustration-aggression hypothesis: displaced aggression
when people are frustrated, they experience a drive to be aggressive towards the object of their frustration
but it is often impossible or inappropriate to behave aggressively towards the source of frustration and as a result any attempt to be aggressive is inhibited
dollard et al. assumed aggression is sometimes displaced from the source of the frustration on to another individual
sometimes referred to ‘kicking the dog’ effect as the individual may have an impulse to attack the source of their frustration but this is impossible so respond by kicking the dog instead to experience catharsis (i.e. scapegoating)
who developed the revised frustration-aggression hypothesis
berkowitz
outline the revised frustration-aggression hypothesis
argued frustration doesn’t always lead to aggression and aggression can occur in the absence of frustration - stated that the likelihood of aggression would change in the presence of certain cues (e.g. weapons)
argued anything that interferes with our ability to reach a goal is experienced as an aversive experience
unanticipated interference with goal attainment is more likely to provoke an aggressive reaction compared to anticipated interference
such unpleasant experiences (e.g. frustration) create ‘negative affect’ in the individual (i.e. negative feelings), such feelings creates the tendency to engage in aggressive behaviour
briefly outline the social learning theory as an explanation of aggression
bandura and walters believed that the specifics of aggressive behaviour are learned by observing models we identify with and imitating behaviours that are seen to be rewarded
outline social learning theory as an explanation of aggression: observation
children primarily learn aggressive responses through observation of a model and then imitating that behaviour
outline social learning theory as an explanation of aggression: observation - vicarious reinforcement
children observe and learn about consequences of aggressive behaviour by watching others being reinforced or punished
witness many examples of aggression at home, school, television and films
by observing the consequences of aggressive behaviour for those who use it, a child gradually learns something about what is considered appropriate and effective conduct in the world around them
so learn behaviours via observation and whether/when such behaviours are worth repeating via vicarious reinforcement
outline social learning theory as an explanation of aggression: mental representations
bandura claimed that in order for social learning to take place, the child must form mental representations of events in their social environment
representations incl possible rewards and punishments of their aggressive behaviour in terms of expectancies of future outcomes
when appropriate opportunity arises, the child will display the learned behaviour as long as the expectation of reward is greater than the expectation of punishment
learn rules of conduct from those around them, such script (rules) becomes internalised and the pattern of behaviour can become a way of life
outline social learning theory as an explanation of aggression: production of behaviour - maintenance through direct experiences
if a child is rewarded for a behaviour, he or she is likely to repeat the same action in similar situations in the future
child who has a history of successfully bullying other children will come to attach considerable value to aggression
outline social learning theory as an explanation of aggression: production of behaviour - self efficacy expectancies
develop confidence in their ability to carry out the necessary aggressive actions
children for whom this form of behaviour has been particularly disastrous in the past have less confidence in their ability to use aggression successfully to resolve conflicts so may turn to other means
outline research into social learning theory as an explanation of aggression: bandura et al. - aim
to investigate if social behaviours (aggression) can be acquired by observation and imitation
outline research into social learning theory as an explanation of aggression: bandura et al. - method
male and female 3-5 year olds
50% exposed to adult model being aggressive towards life-sized bobo doll and other 50% exposed to models that were non-aggressive
model displayed distinctive physically aggressive acs towards the doll e.g. striking on the head with a mallet and verbal aggression (pow)
following exposure, children were shown attractive toys but they weren’t allowed to play with them so became frustrated
then taken to a room where among toys there was a bobo doll
outline research into social learning theory as an explanation of aggression: bandura et al. - findings
children in aggressive condition reproduced physical and verbal aggression behaviour
children in non-aggressive exhibited no physical and verbal aggression
⅓ of children in aggressive condition imitate verbal remarks of the models
boys reproduced more imitative physical aggression than girls but no difference in imitation of verbal aggression
what is meant by deindividuation
refers to a state in which individuals have lower self-awareness and a weaker sense of personal responsibility for their actions, may result from the relative anonymity of being part of a crowd
outline the concept of deindividuation as an explanation of aggression
based on le bon’s classic crowd theory
le bon described how an individual was transformed when part of a crowd
in a crowd, the combination of anonymity, suggestibility and contagion mean that a ‘collective mind’ takes possession of the individual
as a consequence, the individual loses self-control and becomes capable of acting in a way that goes against personal or social norms
outline the nature of deindividuation as an explanation of aggression: festinger et al.
described deindividuation as a psychological state in which inner restraints are lost when ‘individuals are not seen or paid attention to as people’
outline the nature of deindividuation as an explanation of aggression: zimbardo
argued that the psychological state of deindividuation is aroused when individuals join crowds or large groups that act as a ‘cloak of anonymity’
such cloak diminishes any personal consequences for their actions
factors that contribute to this state of deindividuation include anonymity (e.g. wearing a uniform) and altered consciousness due to drugs or alcohol
outline the process of deindividuation as an explanation of aggression
people normally refrain from acting in an aggressive manner partly as there are social norms inhibiting such ‘uncivilised’ behaviour and as they are easily identifiable
being anonymous (so unaccountable) in a crowd has the psychological consequence of reducing inner restraints and increasing behaviours that are usually inhibited
being part of a crowd can diminish awareness of our own individuality as each person is faceless and anonymous (the larger the group, the greater the anonymity)
diminishes fear of the negative evaluation of actions, so weaken the normal barrier to anti-social behaviour that are based on guilt or shame
outline the research into deindividuation as an explanation of aggression: stanford prison experiment (zimbardo)
found that the ‘guards’ (played in a deindividuated state) acted aggressively towards the ‘prisoners’
guards wore mirrored sunglasses to accentuate their deindividuated state
outline the research into deindividuation as an explanation of aggression: zhong
found that wearing mirrored sunglasses male people feel greater anonymity which in turn increase the experience of deindividuation
outline the research into deindividuation as an explanation of aggression: zimbardo - aim
to investigate whether there is a relationship between the process of deindividuation and aggression
outline the research into deindividuation as an explanation of aggression: zimbardo - method
groups of 4 female undergraduates were required to deliver electric shocks to another student to ‘aid learning’
½ of the participants wore bulky lab coats and hoods that hid their faces, sat in separate cubicles and never referred to by name
other ½ wore normal clothes, given large name tags, were introduced to each other by name and able to see each other when seated at the shock machines
outline the research into deindividuation as an explanation of aggression: zimbardo - findings
participants in the deindividuation condition were more likely to press the shock button and for twice as long compared to identifiable participants
what is meant by institutional aggression
refers to aggressive acts that are found in particular violent institutions such as prisons
identify the situational and dispositional explanations of institutional aggression in prisons
deprivation model and importation model respectively
briefly outline the situational explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: the deprivation model
Paterline and Peterson argued that institutional aggression is the product of the stressful and oppressive conditions of the prison itself, rather than the characteristics of an individual
inmates may cope with the pains of imprisonment by choosing to rebel in the form of violence against other prisoners or staff
outline the situational explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: the deprivation model - specific deprivations
Sykes identified deprivations that increase aggression
deprived of security (heightened sense of physical threat), goods, relationships, autonomy (few choices) and liberty (little freedom)
outline research into the situational explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: kimmet and martin
studied over 200 prison inmates, discovering that violence in prison is frequently a way of surviving the risk of exploitation (by appearing weak)
found that most violent situations in prisons were more to do with non-material interests such as the need for respect and fairness or as a way of expressing loyalty and honour
outline the situational explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: the role of prison characteristics
Cooke et al. claimed that ‘violent prisoners are only violent in certain circumstances’
overcrowding
heat and noise
job burnout
outline the situational explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: the role of prison characteristics - overcrowding
Ministry of Justice found that record rates of murder, suicide and assaults were linked to increased overcrowding in prisons
outline the situational explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: the role of prison characteristics - heat and noise
prisons tend to be hot and noisy places so high temperatures and noise exacerbate the effects of overcrowding and may predispose inmates to aggressive behaviour
outline the situational explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: the role of prison characteristics - job burnout
prison staff being psychologically worn out and exhausted from a job and a gradual loss of caring about the people with whom they work (Maslach et al.)
briefly outline a strength of the situational explanation of institutional aggression: research support
McCorkle et al. studied 371 prisons, found overcrowding with a lack of privacy and security significantly increased violence whilst prisons that had vocational programmes reduced violence
led to applications: Wilson found lower temperatures and noise with increased space eradicated violence in the HMP Woodhill, so can be used to improve prisons
briefly outline a criticism of the situational explanation of institutional aggression: challenging evidence
Harer and Steffensmeier collected data from more than 24,000 inmates from 58 prisons, included importation variables (e.g. race and criminal history) and deprivation variables (staff-to-prisoner ratio and security level)
found that race, age and criminal history were the only significant predictors of prison violence whilst none of the deprivation variables were significant
briefly outline the dispositional explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: the importation model
Irwin and Cressey argued that inmates are not ‘blank slates’ when they enter, instead, they bring in their violent pasts and experiences into the prison where toughness and physical exploitation are important survival skills
outline the dispositional explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: gang membership
Allender and Marcel found pre-prison gang memberships appears to be an important determinant of prison misconduct
violence is seen as the social norm as it is was rewarded by other members as increases status and avoid exploitation etc.
known as ‘code of the streets’
outline the dispositional explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: role of dispositional characteristics - anger, antisocial personality type and impulsivity
Wang and Diamond found that these characteristics were stronger predictors of institutional aggression than ethnicity and type of offence committed
anger was the best predictor as predisposed inmates to behave violently
outline the dispositional explanation of institutional aggression in prisons: role of dispositional characteristics - low self control
DeLisi et al. found that low self-control, particularly the tendency to lose one's temper easily was a significant predictor of aggressive behaviour both before and during incarceration
may be learnt through aggressive role models or MAOA-L variant
briefly outline a strength of the dispositional explanation of aggression: research support
Mears et al. measured the code belief and experience of inmates from deprived areas, found higher levels of violence was positively correlated with lack of family support, strong belief in the code and gang membership
BUT DeLisi found inmates with prior street gang involvement were no more likely than other inmates to engage in violence
lack of association can be explained by the violent gang members tending to be isolated from the general inmate population, restricting opportunities for violence (Fischer)
briefly outline a solution of the explanations of institutional aggression: holism
Jiang and Fisher-Giorlando suggested that the importation model is a better explanation of violence between inmates whilst the deprivation model is more useful in understanding inmate aggression against staff
Dobbs and Waid argued as inmates enter prison for the first time they will suffer from deprivation (due to unfamiliarity) but this doesn’t necessarily lead to aggression until it combines with individual characteristics imported into prison by inmates
Dilulio suggested the ‘administrative control model’ that takes into account the combined effect of poor prison management and pre-existing behaviours
what is meant by media influences
changes in behaviour that are attributed to exposure to media, such as television and computer games
outline lab research into the media influences, particularly violent films and television, on aggression: Bjorkqvist
exposed 5 to 6 years olds to either violent or non-violent films, finding that children who had watched the violent film were subsequently rated much higher on measures of physical aggression
outline longitudinal research into the media influences, particularly violent films and television, on aggression: Huesmann et al.
studied 557 children in 1977, and then 329 of these 15 years later
finding that habitual early exposure to violent television was predictive of later adult aggression
relationship persisted even when the possible effects of differences in parenting styles etc. were controlled
outline research into the media influences, particularly violent computer games, on aggression: Porter and Starcevic
interactive violence in video games exerts a greater influence on aggression as individual plays a more active role e.g. first person shooter games
games often reward aggression to progress through the game, portraying it as appropriate and effective
outline lab research into the media influences, particularly violent computer games, on aggression: Anderson and Dill
found that participants blasted their opponents with white noise for longer and rated themselves higher on a hostility scale
after playing a violent ‘first person shooter’ game compared to a slow-paced puzzle game
outline longitudinal research into the media influences, particularly violent computer games, on aggression: Anderson et al.
surveyed 430 children gaining self reports from peers/teachers etc.
found children who had high exposure to violent video games became more verbally and physically aggressive
outline research into the media influences, particularly violent computer games, on aggression: Greitemeyer and Mugge
meta analysis of 98 studies into the effect of violent games in which the goal is to harm someone against prosocial games in which the goal is to benefit someone
found that violent video game use was linked to an increase in aggression and a decrease in prosocial behaviour
identify the three explanations of media influences on aggression
desensitisation
disinhibition
cognitive priming
outline desensitisation as an explanation of media influences on aggression
repeated exposure to violent media, the more acceptable aggressive behaviour becomes due to lowered emotional response of anxiety/fear
becomes perceived as ‘normal’ (not negative) so more likely to engage in aggression (Muller and Linz)
desensitisation occurs due to reduced physiological arousal and cognitive reaction to violence (Linz et al.)
outline disinhibition as an explanation of media influences on aggression
exposure to violent media legitimises aggression as it undermines the social sanctions that usually inhibit such ‘drive to act’ to aggressively
prolonged exposure causes violence to be perceived as acceptable, normal part of life
when violence is justified or left unpunished, concerns about consequences of violence are reduced