Evaluative Conditioning (Week 9)
Part 1 – Introduction to Evaluative Conditioning
Evaluative Conditioning (EC)
- Process of conditioning likes/dislikes toward an otherwise neutral conditioned stimulus (CS) by pairing it with a positive or negative unconditioned stimulus (US).
- Responsible for many personal preferences (e.g., food, music).
- Parallel to classical (Pavlovian) conditioning but:
- Classical conditioning → pairs CS with biologically important US to elicit reflexive conditioned response (CR).
- Evaluative conditioning → pairs CS with affectively valenced US to change attitude/valence toward CS rather than a reflex.
- Formal contingencies:
- \text{CS} + \text{Positive\ US} \rightarrow \text{CS\ Liked}
- \text{CS} + \text{Negative\ US} \rightarrow \text{CS\ Disliked}
Illustrative questions & everyday examples
- “Which tea do you prefer?” shows attitude formation.
- Red Bull can design: pairing can heighten positive evaluation of product.
- Facebook post with humorous meme: pairing upbeat text with brand can transfer positive affect.
Advertising & Politics
- Celebrity endorsements, restaurant décor, political stage-craft all exploit association for attitude shaping.
- “Mere exposure in advertising”: repeated non-valenced exposures alone can also raise liking, but EC leverages valenced co-occurrences for stronger effects.
- Negative example: “Kill the messenger” – dislike for bearer of bad news.
- Actors who portray villains may be personally disliked (“YOU WANTED HER DEAD MORE THAN VOLDEMORT”).
Picture-Picture Paradigm (Levey & Martin, 1975)
- Methodology
- Phase 1: Rate 50 paintings as liked, disliked, or neutral.
- Select two most liked + two most disliked images as USs; several neutrals become CSs.
- Phase 2: Present CS–US pairs 20 × each (e.g., Liked US ↔ Neutral CS, Disliked US ↔ Neutral CS, plus Neutral–Neutral control).
- Phase 3: Re-rate all pictures.
- Key finding: Previously neutral CSs
- Paired with liked US → significant increased liking.
- Paired with disliked US → significant decreased liking.
Part 2 – Generality of Evaluative Conditioning
Early verbal demonstrations
- Razran (1954) – “Conditioned evocation of attitudes.”
- Staats & Staats (1957)
- Nonsense syllables (e.g., QUG, YOF, XEH, LAJ, WUH, GIW) used as CSs.
- Each CS paired with positive or negative English words (USs), e.g., \text{YOF} \rightarrow \text{"beauty, success, money"} versus \text{XEH} \rightarrow \text{"worthless, sick, thief"}.
- Post-pairing, syllables assume corresponding valence.
Visual domain
- Replication: Levey & Martin paradigm above; robust EC with pictures.
Gustatory domain
- Zellner et al. (1983) – Flavoured teas
- CS+: Tea 1 + sugar; CS-: Tea 2 alone.
- Post-test: Greater preference for CS+.
- Baeyens et al. (1990) – Fruit flavours
- Positive condition: flavour + sugar vs. flavour alone.
- Negative condition: flavour + Tween (bitter surfactant) vs. flavour alone.
- Positive shift less reliable because (1) intrinsic pleasant tastes less extreme, (2) evolutionary negative-learning bias.
Cross-modal domain
- Todrank et al. (1995)
- Phase 1: Rate odours & faces separately.
- Phase 2: Present random face-odour pairs.
- Phase 3: Faces re-rated – valence shifts toward that of paired odour.
Biologically significant USs
- Zanna et al. (1970)
- CS+ word paired with electric shock; CS- word alone.
- Later, CS+ words evaluated more negatively even though shock is primary US.
- Affective Priming Task (general method)
- Prime stimulus presented milliseconds before target CS.
- Response latency in categorising valence of target is faster when prime and target share valence—shows implicit EC even with strong biological USs.
Part 3 – Functional Characteristics of EC
Extinction (De Houwer et al., 2000)
- Condition group: 8\times \text{CS–US} pairings → immediate test.
- Extinction group: 8\times \text{CS–US} then 5\times \text{CS alone}.
- Finding: EC effects persist; attitude change is resistant to extinction compared with classical CRs.
Statistical contingency (Baeyens et al., 1993)
- Perfect: Only \text{CS} \rightarrow \text{US}.
- Partial: CS sometimes alone (equal CS-only trials).
- Composite: CS-only & US-only trials added.
- Result: EC occurs even with degraded contingency; mere co-occurrence suffices, unlike predictive learning.
Contingency vs. demand awareness (Field, 2000; Baeyens et al., 1990)
- Demand awareness: Participant guesses experimenter’s aim.
- Contingency awareness: Participant can explicitly report CS–US pairings.
- Findings:
- EC can arise without demand awareness.
- Size of EC effect not strongly linked to contingency awareness (e.g., 77 % vs. 18 % CS–US memory groups showed comparable shifts).
Counterconditioning (Baeyens et al., 1989)
- Baseline ratings → Acquisition (10× CS–US) →
- Counter group: CS paired with opposite-valence US.
- Control: No further pairings.
- Extinction: CS alone.
- Counterconditioning can reverse valence, but initial learning shows persistence.
Post-acquisition US revaluation (Baeyens et al., 1992)
- After CS–US pairing, US itself is re-rated (inflated or deflated) without presenting CS.
- CS valence shifts correspondingly → supports idea that CS taps stored representation of US value rather than simple stimulus–response chaining.
Part 4 – Theoretical Models of Evaluative Conditioning
1. Conceptual-Categorisation Account (Davey, 1994)
- EC = concept learning.
- CS contains many features; pairing highlights US-congruent features, causing recategorisation.
- Problems:
- Cross-modal EC (odours→faces) hard to explain via feature overlap.
- Fails to predict US revaluation effects (shouldn’t matter once CS recategorised).
2. Holistic Account (Martin & Levey, 1970s)
EC = basic primitive mechanism; classical conditioning requires higher cognition.
CS–US presented together → single holistic memory trace including affect.
CS later re-activates full trace → valence retrieval.
Explains: extinction-resistance, contingency-awareness independence, US revaluation.
Weakness: cannot deal with sensory preconditioning.
- Sensory preconditioning demonstration (Hammerl & Grabitz, 1996):
- Phase 1: \text{CS}1\text{ – }\text{CS}2 association.
- Phase 2: \text{CS}_2 – \text{US} pairing.
- Test: \text{CS}_1 acquires valence despite never co-occurring with US.
3. Referential Account (Baeyens et al., 1992)
- Distinguishes signal learning (classical) vs. referential learning (EC).
- Classical: Learner encodes expectancy that US will occur → drives preparatory CR and needs awareness.
- Evaluative: Learner encodes that CS “refers to” valenced US; does not need predictive contingency or awareness.
- Provides conceptual split:
- Classical conditioning: \text{CS} \Rightarrow \text{Expect US}.
- EC: \text{CS} \Rightarrow \text{Refers to good/bad}.
Comparative summary of models
- All capture some empirical regularities (resistance to extinction, lack of contingency awareness requirement, US revaluation, etc.) but none integrate boundary conditions fully.
- Still largely descriptive; neural mechanisms, evolutionary rationales remain active research areas.
Practical, Ethical & Real-World Relevance
- Marketing & consumer manipulation: strategic pairing can mold preferences subconsciously.
- Political campaigns: flags, music, or popular figures adjacent to candidate imagery.
- Clinical possibilities: counterconditioning may help reduce phobias or prejudices.
- Ethical caution: covert affective manipulation can violate informed-consent norms.
Numerical / Methodological Quick Reference
- Repetition typical range: 10\text{ – }20 pairings sufficient for robust EC.
- Liking scales: -100 \text{ (max dislike)} \rightarrow +100 \text{ (max like)}.
- Affective-priming RT facilitation often \sim20\text{–}50\,\text{ms} when prime & target share valence.
- Contingency-awareness tasks: forced-choice memory accuracy compared to 50\% chance.
Concise Take-Home Points
- Preferences can be conditioned through mere co-occurrence with valenced stimuli; predictive contingency and voluntary awareness are not required.
- EC effects are generally durable, cross-modal, and observed with both secondary (pictures, words) and primary (taste, shock) USs.
- Extinction and contingency manipulations weaken but rarely abolish learned valence.
- Competing theoretical accounts differ on whether EC is associative (referential or holistic) or conceptual; sensory-preconditioning evidence challenges a purely holistic view.
- Understanding EC is vital for fields from advertising ethics to clinical psychology.