32 /Introduction to Emotion

Emotion, Arousal, Behavior, and Cognition

How Arousal, Expressive Behavior, and Cognition Interact in Emotion

  • Motivated behavior is often connected to powerful emotions.

  • Emotions are subjective but real.

  • Lisa Feldman Barrett: Experiencing an emotion (e.g., anger) is real.

  • Emotions are our body's adaptive response to ensure we do what is best for us (France de Wall).

  • Emotions focus our attention and energize our actions when facing challenges (Citizen Smith).

  • Emotional stress involves integrating data from our environment, body, and experiences (Francis).

  • Positive emotions can lead to tears of joy, triumphant gestures, and newfound confidence.

  • Negative and prolonged emotions can harm our health.

  • Emotions involve:

    • Bodily arousal (e.g., heart pounding).

    • Expressive behaviors (e.g., quickened pace).

    • Conscious experience (e.g., feelings of panic, fear, joy).

  • Psychological phenomena like vision, sleep, memory, and sex can be approached physiologically, behaviorally, and cognitively.

  • Early emotion researchers addressed two key questions:

    • Does bodily arousal come before or after emotional feelings?

    • How do thinking (cognition) and feeling interact?

James-Lange Theory: Arousal Comes Before Emotion

  • Common sense suggests we cry because we are sad, lash out because we are angry, tremble because we are afraid.

  • William James proposed that this view is backward.

  • James-Lange Theory: We feel sorry because we cry, angry because we strike, afraid because we tremble (James).

  • Emotions result from attention to our bodily activity.

  • The theory was also proposed by Danish physiologist Karl Lange.

  • Example: Noticing a racing heart and shaking with fright leads to feeling fear.

  • Joy is expressed because we are smiling with our teammates.

Cannon-Bard Theory: Arousal and Emotion Occur Simultaneously

  • Physiologist Walter Cannon disagreed with the James-Lange theory.

  • Bodily responses (heart rate, perspiration, body temperature) are too similar and change too slowly to cause different emotions.

  • Cannon-Bard Theory: Bodily responses and experienced emotions occur separately but simultaneously.

  • The triggering stimulus travels to the sympathetic nervous system, causing bodily arousal, and to the brain's cortex, causing awareness of the emotion.

  • Pounding heart does not cause fear, nor does fear cause a pounding heart.

  • Studies of people with spinal cord injuries:

    • Lower spine injuries (loss of sensation only in legs): little change in emotion intensity.

    • High spinal cord injury (loss of sensation below the neck): reported changes in emotions.

Schachter-Singer Two-Factor Theory: Arousal Plus Label Equals Emotion

  • Thinking and feeling interact; how we interpret experiences matters.

  • Schachter-Singer Two-Factor Theory: Emotions have two ingredients: physical arousal and cognitive appraisal.

  • Emotional experience requires a conscious interpretation of arousal.

Spillover Effect
  • Arousal spills over from one event to the next.

  • Example: Receiving news of a dream job after an invigorating run might lead to feeling more elated than if hearing the news after being awake all night studying.

  • Arousal from a soccer match can fuel anger, leading to rioting or violent confrontations.

Experiment
  • College men were injected with epinephrine, triggering arousal.

  • One group was told to expect arousal; the other was told it would help test their eyesight.

  • Participants who were not expecting the arousal caught the apparent emotion of the other person in the waiting room (euphoric or irritated).

  • A stirred-up state can be experienced as one emotion or another, depending on how we interpret and label it.

  • Arousal fuels emotion; cognition channels it.

  • Emotion = arousal + interpretation

Influence of Drugs
  • We can influence how we feel by deliberately changing brain chemistry (e.g., with coffee, nicotine, alcohol).

  • Drugs stimulate the limbic system.

  • Amphetamine experiment:

    • Volunteers were told they were given a placebo or a stimulant (amphetamine), but all received amphetamine.

    • Those expecting a placebo interpreted sensations negatively (jittery, anxious), while those expecting a stimulant recognized the sensations and enjoyed the effect (energized, focused).

    • The basic physiological responses involved in emotion are only part of the experience; the person's understanding and interpretation of the situation strongly influence the emotion.

Zajonc and Lazarus: Cognition and Emotion

Zajonc
  • Emotional reactions can occur apart from or before conscious interpretation of a situation.

  • We may like or dislike something immediately without knowing why.

  • People prefer stimuli they have seen before, even if flashed subliminally (unaware of having previously seen them).

  • We have an automatic radar for emotionally significant information.

  • A subliminally flashed stimulus can prime us to feel better or worse about a follow-up stimulus.

Lazarus
  • The brain processes vast amounts of information without conscious awareness.

  • Some emotional responses do not require conscious thinking.

  • Appraisal: To know whether a stimulus is good or bad, the brain must have some idea of what it is.

  • Emotions arise when we appraise an event as harmless or dangerous.

Neural Pathways of Emotions

  • Emotional responses can follow two different brain pathways.

High Road
  • For complex feelings like hatred and love.

  • The stimulus travels via the thalamus to the brain's cortex.

  • Analyzed and labeled before the response command is sent out via the amygdala (emotion control center).

Low Road
  • For simple likes, dislikes, and fears.

  • A neural shortcut bypasses the cortex.

  • A fear-provoking stimulus travels from the eye or ear, via the thalamus directly to the amygdala.

  • Enables grease-lightning emotional response before intellect intervenes.

  • The amygdala's reactions are so fast that we may be unaware of what has transpired.

  • A conscious fear experience occurs as we become aware that our brain has detected danger.

  • The amygdala sends more neural projections up to the cortex than it receives back.

  • In the forest, we can jump at the sound of rustling bushes before our cortex decides whether it was a snake or the wind.

  • Some of our emotional reactions involve no deliberate thinking.

Summary of Emotion Theories

  • Some simple emotional responses involve no conscious thinking (Zajonc, LeDoux).

  • Automatic perception can influence political decisions.

Table 32.1: Summary of Emotion Theories
  • James-Lange: Emotions arise from awareness of specific bodily responses to stimuli (e.g., observe heart racing, then feel afraid).

  • Cannon-Bard: Emotion-arousing stimuli trigger bodily responses and simultaneous subjective experience (e.g., heart races and feel afraid at the same time).

  • Schachter-Singer: Experience of emotion depends on general arousal and a conscious cognitive label (e.g., interpret arousal as fear or excitement).

  • Zajonc, LeDoux: Some embodied responses happen instantly without conscious appraisal (e.g., automatically feel startled by a sound).

  • Lazarus: Cognitive appraisal (dangerous or not) defines emotion, sometimes without our awareness (e.g., sound is just the wind).

  • Complex feelings are affected by conscious and unconscious information processing, memories, expectations, and interpretations.

  • We have more conscious control over complex emotions.

  • Reappraisal: Changing our interpretations when feeling emotionally overwhelmed.

  • Reappraisal reduces distress and improves school performance.

  • Embrace stress and approach exams with the view that stress helps maintain focus and solve problems.

  • Automatic emotion and conscious thinking weave the fabric of our emotional lives.

Embodied Emotion

  • Emotions involve the body.

  • Some physical responses are easy to notice; others occur without awareness.

Distinct Emotions
  • Most emotion scientists agree that anger, fear, disgust, sadness, and happiness are basic human emotions (Ekman).

  • Carol Izard isolated 10 basic emotions: joy, interest/excitement, surprise, sadness, anger, disgust, contempt, fear, shame, and guilt, most present in infancy.

  • As many as 28 emotions exist, including awe, love, and pride (Cowen and Keltner).

  • Emotions are categorized along two dimensions: positive versus negative, and arousal (low versus high).

Emotions and the Autonomic Nervous System (ANS)

  • In a crisis, the sympathetic division of the ANS mobilizes the body for action.

  • Triggers adrenal glands to release stress hormones (epinephrine/adrenaline and norepinephrine/noradrenaline).

  • Liver pours extra sugar (glucose) into the bloodstream.

  • Respiration increases to supply needed oxygen.

  • Heart rate and blood pressure increase.

  • Digestion slows, diverting blood to muscles.

  • Pupils dilate, letting in more light.

  • Body perspires to cool down.

  • Blood clots more quickly if wounded.

  • The parasympathetic division calms the body when the crisis passes.

  • Without conscious effort, the body's response to danger is coordinated and adaptive, preparing to fight or flee.

  • Different emotions may have distinct arousal fingerprints, but discerning physiological differences among fear, anger, and sexual arousal is difficult.

Physiology of Emotions

  • Different emotions can share common biological signatures.

  • A single brain region can serve as the seat of seemingly different emotions.

  • The insula is activated when we experience negative social emotions, such as lust, pridefulness, and disgust.

  • It is also active when people bite into or think about disgusting food, or feel moral disgust.

  • Similar multitasking regions are found in other brain areas.

  • Scary thrills, elated excitement, and panicky fear involve similar physiological arousal.

  • Some emotions have distinct brain circuits.

  • Observers watching fearful faces show more amygdala activity than those viewing angry faces.

  • Positive moods trigger more left frontal lobe activity.

  • People with positive personalities show more activity in the left frontal lobe.

  • We cannot easily see differences in emotions from tracking heart rate, breathing, and perspiration, but facial expressions and brain activity can vary with the emotion.