Motivated behavior is often connected to powerful emotions.
Emotions are subjective but real.
Lisa Feldman Barrett: Experiencing an emotion (e.g., anger) is real.
Emotions are our body's adaptive response to ensure we do what is best for us (France de Wall).
Emotions focus our attention and energize our actions when facing challenges (Citizen Smith).
Emotional stress involves integrating data from our environment, body, and experiences (Francis).
Positive emotions can lead to tears of joy, triumphant gestures, and newfound confidence.
Negative and prolonged emotions can harm our health.
Emotions involve:
Bodily arousal (e.g., heart pounding).
Expressive behaviors (e.g., quickened pace).
Conscious experience (e.g., feelings of panic, fear, joy).
Psychological phenomena like vision, sleep, memory, and sex can be approached physiologically, behaviorally, and cognitively.
Early emotion researchers addressed two key questions:
Does bodily arousal come before or after emotional feelings?
How do thinking (cognition) and feeling interact?
Common sense suggests we cry because we are sad, lash out because we are angry, tremble because we are afraid.
William James proposed that this view is backward.
James-Lange Theory: We feel sorry because we cry, angry because we strike, afraid because we tremble (James).
Emotions result from attention to our bodily activity.
The theory was also proposed by Danish physiologist Karl Lange.
Example: Noticing a racing heart and shaking with fright leads to feeling fear.
Joy is expressed because we are smiling with our teammates.
Physiologist Walter Cannon disagreed with the James-Lange theory.
Bodily responses (heart rate, perspiration, body temperature) are too similar and change too slowly to cause different emotions.
Cannon-Bard Theory: Bodily responses and experienced emotions occur separately but simultaneously.
The triggering stimulus travels to the sympathetic nervous system, causing bodily arousal, and to the brain's cortex, causing awareness of the emotion.
Pounding heart does not cause fear, nor does fear cause a pounding heart.
Studies of people with spinal cord injuries:
Lower spine injuries (loss of sensation only in legs): little change in emotion intensity.
High spinal cord injury (loss of sensation below the neck): reported changes in emotions.
Thinking and feeling interact; how we interpret experiences matters.
Schachter-Singer Two-Factor Theory: Emotions have two ingredients: physical arousal and cognitive appraisal.
Emotional experience requires a conscious interpretation of arousal.
Arousal spills over from one event to the next.
Example: Receiving news of a dream job after an invigorating run might lead to feeling more elated than if hearing the news after being awake all night studying.
Arousal from a soccer match can fuel anger, leading to rioting or violent confrontations.
College men were injected with epinephrine, triggering arousal.
One group was told to expect arousal; the other was told it would help test their eyesight.
Participants who were not expecting the arousal caught the apparent emotion of the other person in the waiting room (euphoric or irritated).
A stirred-up state can be experienced as one emotion or another, depending on how we interpret and label it.
Arousal fuels emotion; cognition channels it.
Emotion = arousal + interpretation
We can influence how we feel by deliberately changing brain chemistry (e.g., with coffee, nicotine, alcohol).
Drugs stimulate the limbic system.
Amphetamine experiment:
Volunteers were told they were given a placebo or a stimulant (amphetamine), but all received amphetamine.
Those expecting a placebo interpreted sensations negatively (jittery, anxious), while those expecting a stimulant recognized the sensations and enjoyed the effect (energized, focused).
The basic physiological responses involved in emotion are only part of the experience; the person's understanding and interpretation of the situation strongly influence the emotion.
Emotional reactions can occur apart from or before conscious interpretation of a situation.
We may like or dislike something immediately without knowing why.
People prefer stimuli they have seen before, even if flashed subliminally (unaware of having previously seen them).
We have an automatic radar for emotionally significant information.
A subliminally flashed stimulus can prime us to feel better or worse about a follow-up stimulus.
The brain processes vast amounts of information without conscious awareness.
Some emotional responses do not require conscious thinking.
Appraisal: To know whether a stimulus is good or bad, the brain must have some idea of what it is.
Emotions arise when we appraise an event as harmless or dangerous.
Emotional responses can follow two different brain pathways.
For complex feelings like hatred and love.
The stimulus travels via the thalamus to the brain's cortex.
Analyzed and labeled before the response command is sent out via the amygdala (emotion control center).
For simple likes, dislikes, and fears.
A neural shortcut bypasses the cortex.
A fear-provoking stimulus travels from the eye or ear, via the thalamus directly to the amygdala.
Enables grease-lightning emotional response before intellect intervenes.
The amygdala's reactions are so fast that we may be unaware of what has transpired.
A conscious fear experience occurs as we become aware that our brain has detected danger.
The amygdala sends more neural projections up to the cortex than it receives back.
In the forest, we can jump at the sound of rustling bushes before our cortex decides whether it was a snake or the wind.
Some of our emotional reactions involve no deliberate thinking.
Some simple emotional responses involve no conscious thinking (Zajonc, LeDoux).
Automatic perception can influence political decisions.
James-Lange: Emotions arise from awareness of specific bodily responses to stimuli (e.g., observe heart racing, then feel afraid).
Cannon-Bard: Emotion-arousing stimuli trigger bodily responses and simultaneous subjective experience (e.g., heart races and feel afraid at the same time).
Schachter-Singer: Experience of emotion depends on general arousal and a conscious cognitive label (e.g., interpret arousal as fear or excitement).
Zajonc, LeDoux: Some embodied responses happen instantly without conscious appraisal (e.g., automatically feel startled by a sound).
Lazarus: Cognitive appraisal (dangerous or not) defines emotion, sometimes without our awareness (e.g., sound is just the wind).
Complex feelings are affected by conscious and unconscious information processing, memories, expectations, and interpretations.
We have more conscious control over complex emotions.
Reappraisal: Changing our interpretations when feeling emotionally overwhelmed.
Reappraisal reduces distress and improves school performance.
Embrace stress and approach exams with the view that stress helps maintain focus and solve problems.
Automatic emotion and conscious thinking weave the fabric of our emotional lives.
Emotions involve the body.
Some physical responses are easy to notice; others occur without awareness.
Most emotion scientists agree that anger, fear, disgust, sadness, and happiness are basic human emotions (Ekman).
Carol Izard isolated 10 basic emotions: joy, interest/excitement, surprise, sadness, anger, disgust, contempt, fear, shame, and guilt, most present in infancy.
As many as 28 emotions exist, including awe, love, and pride (Cowen and Keltner).
Emotions are categorized along two dimensions: positive versus negative, and arousal (low versus high).
In a crisis, the sympathetic division of the ANS mobilizes the body for action.
Triggers adrenal glands to release stress hormones (epinephrine/adrenaline and norepinephrine/noradrenaline).
Liver pours extra sugar (glucose) into the bloodstream.
Respiration increases to supply needed oxygen.
Heart rate and blood pressure increase.
Digestion slows, diverting blood to muscles.
Pupils dilate, letting in more light.
Body perspires to cool down.
Blood clots more quickly if wounded.
The parasympathetic division calms the body when the crisis passes.
Without conscious effort, the body's response to danger is coordinated and adaptive, preparing to fight or flee.
Different emotions may have distinct arousal fingerprints, but discerning physiological differences among fear, anger, and sexual arousal is difficult.
Different emotions can share common biological signatures.
A single brain region can serve as the seat of seemingly different emotions.
The insula is activated when we experience negative social emotions, such as lust, pridefulness, and disgust.
It is also active when people bite into or think about disgusting food, or feel moral disgust.
Similar multitasking regions are found in other brain areas.
Scary thrills, elated excitement, and panicky fear involve similar physiological arousal.
Some emotions have distinct brain circuits.
Observers watching fearful faces show more amygdala activity than those viewing angry faces.
Positive moods trigger more left frontal lobe activity.
People with positive personalities show more activity in the left frontal lobe.
We cannot easily see differences in emotions from tracking heart rate, breathing, and perspiration, but facial expressions and brain activity can vary with the emotion.