Dr. Hazel Blythehazel.blythe@northumbria.ac.uk
By the end of the session, learners should be able to:
Define concepts of speech, language, and communication with clear examples.
Outline major developmental milestones in the areas of speech, language, and communication, including age ranges for each milestone.
Explain supporting experimental evidence, including seminal studies that have shaped our understanding of language development.
Compare nativist and experiential theoretical views of language development, discussing key proponents of each theory and their implications.
Gaze: Involves looking at certain stimuli to communicate attention and intent. Key for initiating interactions and conveying emotions.
Turn-taking: A fundamental aspect of communication, allowing participants to exchange ideas and maintain a conversation effectively.
Gesture: Includes physical movements such as waving or pointing that complement or replace verbal communication.
Linguistic Universals: Identify properties shared across all human languages, supporting theories of innate language capability.
First Words: Typically emerge around 12 months, marking a significant milestone in communication.
Vocabulary Spurt: An accelerated increase in word acquisition, often seen between 16 and 24 months.
Words to Sentences: Transitioning from single words to two-word phrases occurs around age 2, paving the way for complex sentence structures.
Nativist Theories advocate that language ability is hardwired into humans, suggesting a biological predisposition for grammar and vocabulary acquisition.
Experiential Theories argue that language is learned through interaction with the environment and structured teaching, emphasizing the importance of social context.
Definition: The act of sharing or exchanging information through various channels, including spoken language, writing, and non-verbal cues.
Other mediums: body language, facial expressions, gestures, and even silence can convey meaning.
Definition: A structured system of symbols and signals, with agreed meanings, utilized for communication (e.g., spoken sounds, sign languages, written symbols).
Characteristics: Language is extendable and modifiable, adapting to cultural shifts and personal expression.
Definition: The expression of thoughts, feelings, or ideas verbally through articulate words.
Properties: Involves the vocal mechanism and physiological aspects like breath control and articulatory precision.
Focuses on how information is shared without spoken words, playing a crucial role in understanding and contextualizing verbal communication.
Developmental Order of Gaze:
Initially, gaze follows sounds, illustrating the importance of maternal interaction (Collis & Schaffer, 1975).
Infants can follow gaze starting at 2 months, with improvement as they age (Scaife & Bruner, 1975).
Joint attention is identified as vital for language development, enabling infants to learn from social interactions (Tomasello, 1995).
Joint Attention:
Defined as the coordinated visual attention between a child and an adult on a shared element, crucial for language acquisition.
Attention Switching: Infants are found to label objects based on the gaze direction, highlighting its importance in vocabulary development (Dunham et al., 1993).
Early Instances:
Includes gaze coupling in interactions, showcasing children's developing social skills (Pan & Snow, 1999).
Conversational modeling observed in feeding sessions enhances children's ability to engage in dialogue (Snow, 1977).
Importance of establishing expectations for child responses to foster social and linguistic growth.
Gestures serve as significant communicative tools; their understanding is essential for joint attention and vocabulary enhancement.
Importance of Pointing:
Provides cues for joint focus and is critical for vocabulary acquisition (Harris et al., 1995).
Variations in Gestures:
Some gestures, like waving, are near-universal, while others can carry different meanings depending on the cultural context.
All cultures utilize a language form, and all typically developing individuals will acquire it given appropriate exposure.
Includes varied non-spoken forms such as sign languages that can effectively communicate complex ideas.
Semanticity: The principle that sounds carry meaning, with every language having words linked to concepts.
Arbitrariness: No intrinsic connection between linguistic signs and their meanings.
Displacement: The ability to talk about absent or future events, allowing for complex conversation beyond immediate context.
Productivity: The capacity to create an infinite number of phrases using a limited set of grammatical rules, showcasing human creativity in language use.
Language acquisition evolves through comprehension (understanding) and production (speaking), involving multiple components:
Phonology: Sounds and their components.
Semantics: Meaning of words and sentences.
Grammar: Structure and rules governing language use.
Pragmatics: The context and social rules governing language.
It's been discovered that children begin processing sounds in utero, illustrating the early nature of language engagement (DeCasper & Spence, 1986).
Child-Directed Speech: Utilizes exaggerated expressions, repetition, proximity, and a higher-pitched voice to engage infants.
Non-verbal games (e.g., pat-a-cake) promote social interaction.
Providing dynamic opportunities for vocalizing through mother-infant interactions to simulate turn-taking, enhancing linguistic skills.
The notion of sensitive periods suggests a specific timeframe during which language acquisition is most effective. Age of exposure significantly influences language learning success (Johnson & Newport, 1989).
0 months: Cooing—infants start producing vowel-like sounds.
6 months: Reduplicated babbling—infants produce repetitive consonant-vowel combinations.
12 months: First words emerge, marking the beginning of meaningful speech.
2-5 years: Transition from two-word combinations to more complex multi-word utterances, with increasing syntactical complexity.
Infants can learn the meanings of familiar words as early as 6-9 months (Bergelson & Swingley, 2012).
Receptive language develops before expressive language, as children begin to understand spoken language long before they are capable of verbal communication themselves.
The vocabulary spurt, which can occur between 16 and 24 months, signifies an essential transition in language capability.
Children often utilize holophrases, where a single word conveys an entire idea or phrase.
The growth of vocabulary accelerates during the toddler years, leading to telegraphic speech (two-word utterances) around age 2.
As children mature, they develop intricate sentence structures, relying heavily on their expanding vocabulary and understanding of grammatical constructs (Fenson et al., 1994).
Claims that language acquisition is an inherent ability, suggesting children are born with a natural capacity for language (Pinker, 1994).
Emphasizes that linguistic skills develop efficiently, even when language input is incomplete or imperfect (Saffran et al., 1996).
Proposes that language is acquired through environmental exposure and structured learning.
Highlights that children learn regularities in language through their interactions and experiences, denoting the role of social context in language development.
Evaluation Question: How easy is it for children to learn language? Discuss factors affecting language acquisition and the varying degrees of challenges faced by children in diverse environments.