Video 1
Three videos to be watched, sourced from American news stations
Topics include crime and suspects, specifically sensitive issues like murder and child predators
First case: Jessica Torres, shot and killed in San Bernardino
Occurred during a robbery attempt outside a hamburger stand
Viewing the incident as a murder-for-hire linked to the victim's husband
Investigation reveals additional suspects involved
Police believe the husband paid hundreds of thousands to have his wife killed
All five suspects facing murder charges
Students asked to rate the husband's perceived guilt based on watched videos.
Video 2
New topic: child predator case
Officers tipped off about a man planning to meet a teenage boy outside Glendale Galleria
Meeting was part of a sting operation by "People versus Preds"
Group's aim: lure alleged sexual predators into meetings with supposed underage victims
Man arrested during undercover operation
Adult was communicating online while posing as a child
Rick Chambers reports live from Glendale
Group uses dating apps to pose as underage children to catch predators
Ryan Abdul Rahim allegedly communicated with the group, thinking he was meeting a 14-year-old boy
He was interested in engaging in sexual activities with the alleged minor
Glendale Police notified of the meeting
Abdul Rahim attempted to flee but was caught after a brief chase
He faces three felony charges and was arraigned in court
The group is not acting as agents of the police but reports criminal activity when caught.
Video 3
Incident: San Bernardino County fire named the "Line fire"
Area Affected: Consumed over 35,000 acres
Evacuations: Remain in effect in parts of Highland, Big Bear, and Running Springs
Arrest: Justin Wayne Albert, 34, arrested for intentionally starting the fire
Investigation:
Joint investigation by Highland station detectives and Cal Fire
Videos helped identify the suspect
Search warrant executed at suspect's residence led to detention and evidence collection
Charges: Initially held on $80,000 bail, now no bail after enhancement
Containment Status: 14% contained with ongoing firefighting efforts
Challenges for Firefighters:
Difficult terrain (steep, rocky)
Safety of firefighters prioritized
Damage: One structure confirmed destroyed, further assessments ongoing
Community Safety: Preparations ongoing to protect Big Bear community and evacuations initiated.
Public Call for Information: Authorities seeking more details related to the suspect and potential links to other fires.
News media operates under the principle "if it bleeds, it leads" to attract viewers.
Sensational stories keep audience attention, while mundane topics do not.
Example: Popularity of crime dramas and sensationalized content on platforms like TikTok.
Local news typically prioritizes gruesome and heinous crime stories to maintain viewership.
The portrayal of crime in the media can influence public opinion on the criminal justice system and individuals within it.
Audience opinions vary based on factors such as the type of media consumed and specific cases reported.
News broadcasts
Includes television news, newspapers, social media news accounts, and websites.
Provides information about local and global legal systems.
Focus on Sensationalism:
News often highlights significant crimes, creating a dramatic narrative (e.g., police activity, news helicopters).
Racially biased:
Crime reporting commonly presents racially biased views.
Victims often portrayed as white; offenders more likely to be people of color.
Reinforces pre-existing racial stereotypes and biases.
Implications:
These biases can lead jurors to have explicit or implicit biases during trials, affecting the legal process.
Reality television
The media provides self-explanatory information about the legal system for potential jurors.
Cases are often covered as they progress through the court system and even after they conclude.
News reports highlight recent crimes and possible suspects, following cases throughout their legal journey.
Coverage includes high-profile offenders appearing in court and details about daily court proceedings.
Once a jury reaches a verdict, media reports are ready to inform the public.
The goal is to educate the public about the criminal justice system, how it operates, and ongoing events.
Highly publicized cases can shape public perceptions and attitudes about the effectiveness of the system.
The average person may have limited knowledge of the legal system but can still form opinions based on news snippets.
Public opinions often arise from the media presentation of cases, leading to biases regarding legal processes.
Other media sources also exist beyond news that discuss the criminal justice system and offenders.
Fictional media
Fictional media, especially scripted crime dramas like CSI, depict the criminal justice system and criminals.
These shows present realistic criminal matters despite being fictional, as their cases are not real.
Researchers have analyzed the accuracy of information portrayed in these TV shows regarding the criminal justice system.
Popular types of forensic evidence featured include fingerprint analysis, blood analysis, and DNA testing.
A common theme in these shows is that the offender is usually caught by the end of the episode, which may not reflect reality.
Research indicates that the portrayal of forensic science is often exaggerated and dramatized for television.
For example, DNA evidence is depicted as easily collectible and usable in convictions, which is misleading.
A study in New York City found that DNA was used in only 7% of homicide investigations and did not frequently lead to convictions, contrasting with the portrayal in shows like CSI.
Media impacts understanding of the legal system
Creates biases that jurors may carry into court
Discusses the relationship between media consumption and juror decision-making
Research suggests media intake leads to legally relevant attitudes and perceptions
Example: formation of racially biased views regarding minority groups and crime
Biases brought into the courtroom can affect case outcomes and evidence evaluation
Importance of recognizing the negative real-world impacts of media consumption on legal proceedings.
Racial stereotypes
Heavy news viewers, often older adults, may assume racially unsuccessful suspects are Black.
News media viewers tend to associate Black individuals or minorities with violence more than White individuals.
They perceive minority groups as more criminally culpable, influencing guilt perceptions.
Jurors with racial stereotypes may view Black defendants as more guilty than White defendants, despite identical cases.
Positive perception of suspects
Increased exposure to crime-related media leads to heightened perceptions or concerns about crime.
Concerns about legal matters
Sensationalized stories can shift individuals' perceptions of safety and concern about crime.
Research shows general trends on how media consumption affects jury decision-making.
Viewing negative portrayals of the criminal justice system can lead to distrust and skepticism about its legitimacy.
Pre-trial publicity can significantly influence jurors' decisions.
Real cases and suspects receive media coverage before trials.
Media often portrays prosecution positively while negatively depicting defendants, regardless of actual guilt.
Increased media exposure leads to a presumption of guilt among jurors.
Jurors may misremember pretrial information as trial evidence, affecting decision-making.
Difficulty arises in distinguishing between information from media and evidence presented in court.
Misidentifying sources of information can lead jurors to consider inadmissible material in their deliberations.
Numerous articles exist on the effects of pre-trial publicity, often found in legal textbooks.
Decision to cover one or two random articles in paper.
Article by Custer and Bandit Bluff focused on sexual victimization.
Sexual victimization is common but often sensationalized in media.
Examples include shows like SVU that dramatize sexual offenses.
Researchers investigated the impact of media portrayals on perceptions of sexual violence.
Hypothesis: Regular viewers of SVU perceive greater risk of victimization compared to non-viewers.
Study details:
Sample size: 546 women surveyed with self-administered questionnaires.
Measured daily television viewing hours (average: 2.6 hours).
Focused on genres related to sexual victimization.
Additional measures:
Fear of being victimized.
Perceived risk of victimization.
Perceived control over preventing victimization.
Seriousness of the issue.
Personal experience with crime.
Key findings:
Increased viewing of crime shows correlates with heightened perceived risk of sexual victimization.
Non-viewers perceive lower risk compared to regular viewers.
Stronger correlation noted for women of higher socioeconomic status with no real-life crime experience.
Conclusion: Exposure to crime dramas increases perceived risk of sexual victimization, particularly among specific demographic groups.
CSI Overview: Long-running crime drama TV show with several iterations (CSI version 1 to 5).
Research Focus: Studies have investigated the depiction of DNA evidence in CSI.
Key Study Findings:
Analyzed 51 episodes from four different seasons of CSI.
DNA evidence was searched for in 84% of episodes.
When DNA was searched for, it was found 93% of the time.
DNA analysis led to a match for an actual person 87% of the time.
Unrealistic Expectations:
The frequency of DNA findings and successful matches in the show is far beyond reality (actual DNA finding rates around 7%).
This portrayal creates an unrealistic expectation for viewers about the role of DNA evidence in courtrooms
Impact of CSI and Similar Shows: Viewership of shows relying on DNA evidence is speculated to influence real criminal case decision making.
CSI Effect Defined: Coined by Bradley, it refers to how these shows create unrealistic expectations regarding criminal investigations and forensic evidence.
Influence on Perception: These expectations shape public perceptions of the legal system and jurors' decision-making processes in real cases.
Expectations in Courtroom: Regular viewers may enter the courtroom with the assumption that forensic evidence, especially DNA, is always available and crucial for convictions.
Judicial Concerns: Judges have noted that the CSI Effect complicates convictions since many cases lack DNA evidence, leading jurors to question the credibility of cases without it.
Prosecutors' Observations: Prosecutors report that jurors come with high expectations for forensic evidence and may feel disappointed if such evidence is not presented, affecting their comfort in declaring a defendant guilty.
No DNA, Not Guilty Belief: Judges and prosecutors often hold the belief that a lack of DNA evidence leads to a not guilty verdict.
Defense Effect / Reverse CSI Effect: This concept posits that media portrayal, particularly crime dramas, can increase the chances of convictions by bolstering the credibility of prosecution witnesses, especially those presenting forensic evidence.
Overreliance on Forensic Evidence: There is a tendency to rely heavily on forensic science witnesses, which may increase conviction rates, potentially leading to wrongful convictions.
Prosecution Bias: Crime dramas generally favor the prosecution; if forensic evidence is presented, jurors may assume guilt.
Producer Effect: Viewership of crime dramas may educate laypeople about the criminal justice system, potentially fostering a better understanding of legal processes among jurors.
Diverse Opinions: Opinions on the CSI effect vary:
Some argue it creates challenges in securing convictions.
Others believe it results in excessive convictions based on forensic evidence.
Additionally, the producer effect suggests enhanced public understanding of the system gained through viewership.
Research on the CSI effect shows that viewership may raise expectations for DNA and forensic evidence in trials.
Jury members may expect to see forensic evidence but are often surprised when it is absent.
Lack of DNA evidence does not necessarily prevent jurors from convicting; they may explore other evidence sources.
Findings on the CSI effect are mixed, with some studies showing varying effects on conviction rates while others find no impact.
No clear relationship has been established between media portrayal and jury verdicts.
Evidence does not strongly support the existence of the producer's effect, CSI effect, or reverse CSI effect.
Overall, while there may be an impact, the specific outcomes and influences on jurors remain ambiguous and uncertain.
Unclear impact of media and technology on juror decision-making.
Media such as TV shows build biases among jurors.
Technology is widespread and influences evidence in court.
Film and video evidence has been used in courts for years, not a new concept.
There's been an exponential increase in video evidence in trials.
Evidence variety includes unstaged, unedited footage from multiple sources (e.g., surveillance cameras, police dash cams).
Introduces the concept of "seuss variance": unofficial recordings made by citizens, as opposed to formal surveillance.
Citizens often carry devices capable of recording video/audio, increasing the amount of available evidence.
The psychological impact of video evidence on jurors is significant and multi-faceted.
Video evidence is often presented in court using projectors or TV systems from various sources (e.g., convenience store cameras, police body cams).
Jurors view video evidence as credible and persuasive.
This perception may be linked to processing fluency, where visual stimuli are processed more quickly and effectively than verbal testimonies.
For example, jurors are likely to understand and believe a video of a theft more readily than a witness's verbal account of the same incident.
Visual evidence is often seen as more reliable and representative of reality, leading jurors to trust what they see.
The belief in the accuracy of videos can lead jurors to treat them as definitive proof of events, as if they witnessed the crime themselves.
The "picture superiority effect" indicates that visual evidence is remembered better than verbal evidence by jurors.
Video evidence presented early in a trial is more likely to be recalled during deliberations.
Overall, while video evidence appears to provide benefits in terms of understanding and retention, its influence can complicate juror decision-making.
Video evidence provides a real-world account of events, increasing perceived reliability.
It is considered valuable for establishing liability and probative value in legal contexts.
There are limitations and negative aspects to heavily relying on video evidence.
Focus will be on three types of detrimental influences that video evidence can have on decision-making.
Naive Realism: The first detrimental influence of video evidence.
People often believe videos provide objective, reliable evidence of events as they occurred.
Jurors may assume videos depict reality exactly, akin to being a virtual witness.
Videos may not represent the full picture; factors affecting perception include:
Distance from cameras may obscure details.
Events happening outside the camera’s view.
Important actions occurring before or after the video frames.
Jurors may mistakenly assume they grasp the complete truth from video evidence due to naive realism.
Overconfidence in Video Evidence
Jurors may exhibit overconfidence based on viewing video evidence.
Belief that video equals reality (naive realism).
Example: A juror sees a video of a man stealing and assumes it represents the truth.
This belief can lead to heightened confidence in judgments (e.g., believing the suspect is guilty).
Confidence levels can shift dramatically (from 50% to 100%) after seeing video evidence.
Jurors may overlook other information that contradicts their beliefs due to overconfidence.
Factors such as the quality of video (e.g., blurry footage) can be ignored.
Jurors may dismiss doubts or alternative interpretations, believing their judgment is infallible based on the video.
Overvaluing Video Evidence
Detrimental influence where jurors place excessive importance on video evidence over other forms of evidence presented in a trial.
A juror may assign all judgment value to the video they have seen.
Other evidence may be ignored in favor of the video, affecting the final verdict.
Example: A blurry security cam video shows a masked individual stealing.
Even with contradictory evidence (such as an alibi), jurors might focus solely on the video.
This attitude can skew the assessment of evidence and lead to flawed judgments in trials.
Jurors can perceive video evidence in a biased manner.
Not only do videos create biases, but the viewing process is also influenced by various factors.
Three sources of bias when dealing with video evidence:
Attentional Focus: What jurors concentrate on in the video can skew perceptions.
Prior Beliefs and Attitudes: Existing beliefs before viewing may affect how jurors interpret the evidence.
Current Motivations: Jurors' motivations related to the case can further influence their understanding of the video
Attentional focus is a source of bias in video evidence evaluation.
People attend to visual environments differently; this applies to videos as well.
Selective attention occurs when individuals focus on certain aspects while ignoring others.
Selective attention can impact legal judgments, as demonstrated in studies.
Example: Participants identifying closely with police officers viewed them more favorably in videos.
Judgments can be influenced by what individuals fixate on in the video (e.g., officer vs. offender).
Different jurors may focus on different parts of the same video, leading to varied interpretations.
If jurors cannot attend to all aspects of the video, they may lack critical information for judgment.
Human nature tends towards selective attention based on personal experiences, which can bias understanding of the event depicted in the video.
Prior beliefs and attitudes can lead to biased interpretations of video evidence.
Individuals bring their own predispositional beliefs into the legal environment.
Example from research: Participants subliminally exposed to black male faces more quickly identified an ambiguous object as a handgun compared to those exposed to white faces.
This reflects pre-existing stereotypes associating minority groups with violence.
Study: Participants shown blurry surveillance video with subliminally flashed faces (black or white) before identifying an object.
Those primed with black faces were more likely to perceive the object as a gun; those with white faces perceived it as non-violent.
Relation to juror biases: If a defendant in a police shooting video is black, jurors with racial stereotypes may perceive a blurry object as a gun, influenced by their biases.
Another example: Jurors more in favor of the death penalty tend to judge a suspect in a video as having bad intentions, regardless of the video's content.
Video content remains unchanged, yet jurors' interpretations differ based on their existing beliefs.
Overall, video evidence does not guarantee uniform interpretation due to jurors' prior concepts and attitudes.
Pre-existing biases can significantly affect jurors' understanding and judgment based on the same video evidence.
Current motivations can lead to biased interpretations of video evidence.
Concept: Motivated cognition refers to decision-makers' preferences regarding outcomes.
Individuals are cognitively motivated to reach a desired decision, engaging with information to support that conclusion.
This can result in "wishful seeing" when dealing with visual evidence.
Definition of wishful seeing: viewing ambiguous features in a video as consistent with one's desired outcome.
Example: If a juror believes the defendant is guilty, they may perceive a blurry video as showing a gun.
Conversely, if a juror believes the defendant is innocent, they may interpret the same video as not depicting a threat.
The video and case remain unchanged, but jurors' preconceived ideas influence their perceptions.
Ambiguous stimuli are interpreted in alignment with jurors' motivations, affecting their judgments.
Motivated cognition can lead to biased interpretations based on what individuals want to believe.
Video and audio often accompany each other in court settings.
Audio can influence jurors similarly to video.
Audio can be clear or ambiguous, affecting interpretation.
Examples of audio clarity:
Clear audio: "I will kill you".
Ambiguous audio: unclear threats that could be misinterpreted.
Background noise can complicate audio clarity (e.g., crowded places).
Jurors may attend to audio for context but might misinterpret it.
Phonemic restoration issue: jurors may hear incomplete audio and fill in gaps unconsciously.
Example: Audio saying "good night" may lead jurors to believe they heard "good night" even if they only catch part of it.
Video interrogation
Recommendation for all police interrogations to be video recorded from start to finish.
Purpose: To document the entire interrogation process and any potential confessions.
Ideal outcome: Jurors can view the recording to understand what transpired during the interrogation.
Benefits of recording:
Allows jurors to assess if confessions were coerced.
Helps in evaluating the validity of confessions, especially if the defendant claims coercion (e.g., lack of food/water, long hours).
Emphasis: Full video documentation ensures transparency in the judicial process.
Complexity: Questions arise whether a video truly provides clear evidence of coercion or sketchiness in the interrogation.
Interpretation of video evidence may come with its own set of challenges, affecting juror perceptions and final judgments.