Selective attention is defined as the cognitive process of focusing on a small number of sensory stimuli (both external and internal) while ignoring irrelevant sensory inputs (McLeod, 2018; Murphy et al., 2016).
It helps prevent overload of the informational processing system.
Critical for daily functioning and cognitive processes.
Plays a role in child therapy, particularly in behavior management techniques.
Associated features with ADHD, where children may exhibit executive functioning difficulties alongside selective attention deficits (Brodeur & Pond, 2001).
Bottleneck Theories: Focus on the flow and filtering of information.
Broadbent’s Filter Theory (1958):
Stimuli enter a sensory buffer, which filters based on physical characteristics (e.g., frequency, location).
Selective filter only allows certain stimuli to be processed.
Criticism: The “cocktail party effect” suggests meaning is extracted before filtering.
Late Selection Theory (Deutsch & Deutsch, 1963):
All stimuli analyzed for meaning but not all allowed past the filter; selection occurs based on physical characteristics and relevance of stimuli.
Attenuation Theory (Treisman, 1964):
Stimuli are not filtered but attenuated, meaning they enter the sensory register at a lower intensity.
Unattended messages can be combined and understood in dichotic listening tasks.
Perceptual Load Theory:
Highlights processing demands of tasks.
Irrelevant stimuli are harder to ignore with low perceptual load.
Criticism: Operationalizing perceptual load is challenging, affecting accuracy of research.
Cognitive Load:
Refers to internal resources needed for selective attention.
High cognitive load can lead to increased distraction by irrelevant stimuli.
Dilution Theory (Tsal & Benoni, 2010):
Argues support for perceptual load theory may stem from dilution of irrelevant stimuli interference.
Hybrid Theory:
Integrates aspects of both perceptual load and dilution theories.
Essential to daily functioning, especially in children with ADHD, who show executive functioning deficits.
Older children perform better in selective attention tasks than younger children, linking developmental factors to cognitive load (Brodeur & Pond, 2001).
Intentional Application:
Used in child therapy to reinforce positive behaviors while ignoring negative ones (discriminative attention).
Active ignoring involves withholding attention until problematic behavior ceases, reversing patterns of maladaptive behaviors.
Understanding selective attention is vital for effective cognitive processing and can help in behavior management techniques in pediatric psychology.
Broadbent, D. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon Press.
Brodeur, D. A., & Pond, M. (2001). The development of selective attention in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 29(3), 229–239.
Deutsch, J. A. & Deutsch, O. (1963). Attention: some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70, 80–90.
Girard, E. I., et al. (2018). Parent-child interaction therapy with toddlers. New York, NY: Springer International Publishing.
Lavie, N., & Tsal, Y. (1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 183–197.
McLeod, S. A. (2018). Selective attention. Retrieved from https://www.simplypsychology.org/attention-models.html
Murphy, G., et al. (2016). Twenty years of load theory – Where are we now, and where should we go next? Psychonomics Bulletin and Review, 23(5), 1316–1340.
Patterson, G. R. (1982). A social learning approach: 3. Coercive family process. Eugene: Castalia.
Scalf, P. E., et al. (2013). Competition explains limited attention and perceptual resources: Implications for perceptual load and dilution theories. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 243.
Treisman, A. (1964). Selective attention in man. British Medical Bulletin, 20(1), 12–16.
Tsal, Y., & Benoni, H. (2010). Diluting the burden of load: Perceptual load effects are simply dilution effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1645–1656.