What Is Cognitive Psychology?
Example Scenario:
You’re walking along a dark, unfamiliar city street. It’s raining and foggy, and you are cold and a bit apprehensive. As you walk past a small alley, you catch some movement out of the corner of your eye. You turn to look down the alley and start to make out a shape coming toward you. As the shape draws nearer, you are able to make out more features, and you realize that it’s...
^Cognitive psychology aims to understand this kind of complex mental processing
the branch of psychology that is concerned with how people acquire, store, transform, use, and communicate information
study of mental processes by which humans learn about and interact with the world
Specific Topics
perception
attention
memory
language
problem-solving
planning
influences on cognitive psychology:
philosophical antecedents
philosophical influences on the study of cognition
empiricism vs. nativism
nurture vs. nature
ENVIRONMENT/EXPERIENCES
Aristotle
Locke
“blank slate”
HEREDITY & BIOLOGY
Descartes
Plato
psychological antecedents
Wilhelm Wundt (1832-1920)
structuralism
introspection
William James (1842-1910)
Principles of Psychology (1890)
functionalism
Hermann von Ebbinghaus (1850-1909)
experimental observation
Sigmund Freud (1856-1939)
subconscious
Karl Popper (1902-1994)
philosopher of science
falsifiability
can make progress by disproving theories as well as proving theories
Gestalt Psychology
the whole is greater than the sum of the parts
behaviorism
Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936)
classical conditioning
Thorndike (1874-1949)
instrumental/operant conditioning
reinforcement, punishment
behaviorism takes over American psychology
mental processes unobservable
should simply describe the relationship between stimuli and responses
introspection rejected
concepts such as ID, Ego, and Superego rejected as untestable fantasies
John Broadus Watson (1878-1958)
radical behaviorism
Burhus Frederic Skinner (1904-1990)
continues to push for “radical behaviorism”
all behavior (even language) is the result of classical and instrumental/operant conditioning
“verbal behavior” (1957)
the decline of behaviorism
other science requires unobservable theoretical entities (atoms, genes, gravity)
still passes Popper’s test
Edward C. Tolman (1886-1959)
internal representations
cognitive maps
Noam Chomsky (1928-)
refutes Skinner’s attempts to explain language in “verbal behavior”
critique (1959) had big impact on behaviorism
the post-war context
developments during and after war
engineering
idea of humans as information processors
thinking as computation
development of computers made concept of thinking-as-computation more plausible
artificial intelligence
computers’ ability to do “smart” things (chess)
thinking-as-computation even more plausible
early cognitive psychology
late 1950s, early 1960s
“computer metaphor” is psychology
one type of information processing approach
consequences
architecture of mind
seperate systems
how large
sequences of steps during cognitive processing
format of information
parts of a task
how quickly performed
“information processing” approach
subtypes
1967: first textbook
Cognitive Psychology by Ulric Neisser
cognitive science
intersection of several disciplines including:
psychology
linguistics
philosophy
computer science
computer simulation
neuroscience
aim:
integrate cognitive science, neuroimaging, neuropsychology
computer metaphor gradually losing favor
“network metaphor”
cognitive psychology today
increasing emphasis on:
formal models
neuroscience
fine-grained measures
statistical analyses
still information processing approach
“network” metaphor > computer metaphor
neural information processing and learning
information transmission in the brain
neural basis of learning
information transmission
presynaptic action potential
neurotransmitter release
neurotransmitter binding
ion flow
postsynaptic potential
excitatory postsynaptic potential may - action potential (spike)
inhibitory postsynaptic potential may - prevent spike
information flow
basis of all cognitive (and non-cognitive) processing
determined by synaptic strengths between neurons
changing information flow
change synaptic strengths
synaptic modification
if based on experience
learning
the consequence
next time- postsynaptic action potential more likely
long-term change in response
learning
“long-term potentiation”
brain structures and cognitive processing
major divisions of the brain
brainstem
hindbrain
rhombencephalon
medulla
pons
cerebellum
midbrain
mesencephalon
tectum
tegmentum
reticular formation
forebrain
prosencephalon
basal ganglia
limbic system
thalamus
hypothalamus
cerebral cortex/neocortex
Methods for Linking Brain & Cognition
A. Brain Imaging
B. Neuropsychology
A. BRAIN IMAGING
Brain Imaging Techniques
Electrical Activity in the Brain
Functional Brain Imaging
ELECTRICAL ACTIVITY
Single-Unit Recording
Focuses on action potentials from individual neurons
Includes components:
Display
Electrode
Stimulus
Electrical Signal from Brain
Overview of Brain Imaging Methods
Examines electrical activity in the context of single-unit recording.
From Stimulus to Response
Process Flow: External World → Stimulus → Response
Involves:
Sensory Relay Nuclei
Thalamus
Receptors
Primary Sensory Areas
Secondary Sensory Areas
Primary Motor Cortex
Association Cortex
Motor Neurons
Single-Unit Recording Details
Examines particular processing within tiny brain regions,
Contrasts with multiunit techniques that look at properties of multiple cells.
Learning Check
Single-cell recording enables understanding the response properties of:
C. One or a relatively small number of neurons at a time
The Electroencephalogram (EEG)
Utilizes scalp electrodes to record voltage fluctuations.
Characteristics of EEG
Not specific to individual neurons;
Records waves generated by neurons;
Weaker signals diminish with distance;
Multiple electrodes help triangulate locations;
Characteristic wave patterns identified.
Learning Check
Which statement is INCORRECT?
B. The invasiveness of EEG recording techniques is the same as that of single and multi-unit recording techniques.
Event-Related Potentials (ERPs)
Overview
Patterns of EEG triggered by a stimulus
Embedded within the overall EEG
Example of ERP
P1, N1, P2, N2, P3
Fluctuations illustrated in an averaged ERP waveform.
Single Unit Recording vs. ERP
Single-Unit Recording: Insight into inner workings, individual neurons' responses.
ERP: Aggregated data, equivalent to crowd noise within a stadium.
ERP Study
Example: Processing Emotion Words
Kissler et al. (2007) study on emotional word responses
High emotional words trigger greater responses 200-300 ms in left occipito-temporal areas.
FUNCTIONAL BRAIN IMAGING
Functional Brain Imaging Key Ideas - 1
Increased brain activity correlates with:
Higher oxygen utilization in blood,
Greater changes in blood oxygen content.
Functional Brain Imaging Key Ideas - 2
Methods:
fMRI: Detects magnetic properties of blood
fNIRS: Tracks optical properties of blood
Functional Brain Imaging Key Ideas - Summary
Measures variations in brain activity through blood flow.
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)
Description
Relies on the magnetic properties of oxygenated/deoxygenated hemoglobin in blood.
Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS)
Based on optical properties of blood's hemoglobin.
Comparing ERPs with fMRI/fNIRS
ERP: Excellent temporal resolution; poor spatial resolution.
fMRI: Good spatial resolution; poor temporal resolution.
fNIRS: Similar to ERP in pros and cons.
Learning Check
fMRI and fNIRS enable imaging of:
B. Blood oxygen changes in the brain.
Learning Check
Which statement is CORRECT?
E. fMRI and fNIRS both utilize differences in the properties of oxygenated vs deoxygenated blood flow.
B. NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
Neuropsychology Overview
Study of cognitive processing in patients with brain injuries or deteriorations.
Causes of Brain Injury/Deterioration
Conditions Leading to Brain Damage:
Blood flow reduction
Cerebrovascular disorders/strokes
Head injuries
Tumors
Infections
Degenerative disorders
First Neuropsychological Finding
1861 Discovery by Paul Broca:
Left frontal damage linked to speech impairment: Broca’s aphasia (expressive aphasia), characterized by halting and effortful speech.
Broca's Area
Identified location linked to speech production responsibilities.
Wernicke’s Aphasia
1874 Discovery by Carl Wernicke:
Damage in the temporal lobe affecting comprehension, resulting in fluent yet nonsensical speech (receptive aphasia).
Importance of Broca and Wernicke
Pioneered studies on language-brain-behavior relations and revealed cognitive consequences of brain lesions.
Other Neuropsychological Examples
Spatial Cognition: Right parietal lobe injury disrupts visual-spatial tasks.
Memory: Hippocampal injury alters specific memory types.
Methods for Linking Brain & Cognition
A. Brain Imaging
B. Neuropsychology
Sensation vs. Perception
Sensation: The process of receiving external stimuli through sensory receptors.
Perception: The cognitive process of interpreting what is sensed, allowing us to understand our environment. This process builds upon the sensory input and elaborates it into a comprehensive interpretation.
Constructivist vs. Ecological Views
Constructivist View: Proposes that perception is constructed through cognitive processes, emphasizing the role of mental interpretation and context.
Ecological View: Suggests that perception is inherently linked to a rich environmental structure that can be processed directly, with both traditional (direct perception with no mental processing) and modern (involving cognitive complexity) perspectives.
Neural Bases of Visual Perception
Structure of the Eye
Light enters through the cornea, passes through the pupil (controlled by the iris), and is focused by the lens onto the retina.
The retina contains photoreceptors that convert light into neural signals: cones (for color and detail, primarily located in the fovea) and rods (sensitive to light and motion but color-blind, located in the periphery)
Visual Pathway from Retina to Cortex
Optic Nerve: Transmits visual information from the retina.
Thalamus: Acts as a relay station for sensory information before reaching the cortex.
Primary Visual Cortex (V1): Located in the occipital lobe, responsible for initial visual processing.
Secondary Cortical Areas: Further processing occurs in areas associated with visual perception, with parallel processing pathways identified as the:
Occipital-Parietal Pathway (where): Involved in spatial processing.
Occipital-Temporal Pathway (what): Involved in object recognition.
Organizing the Visual Scene into Objects
1. Perceptual Grouping
The challenge of organizing visual stimuli into distinct objects involves understanding which elements group together and which do not.
Gestalt Principles: Provide rules on how objects are perceived together, emphasizing higher-level organizational principles:
Pragnanz (Simplicity): Visual inputs are interpreted in the simplest way.
Similarity: Objects that are similar tend to be grouped together.
Parallelism & Symmetry: Shapes that are parallel or symmetrical are likely seen as part of the same object.
Proximity: Closer objects are seen as forming a group.
Common Fate: Objects moving in the same direction are grouped.
2. Figure-Ground Organization
This involves segregating a visual scene into a foreground (figure) and background (ground).
Factors that influence figure-ground assignment include geometric cues such as size, symmetry, and familiarity.
Conclusion
Understanding perception involves unpacking both the physiological foundations and the cognitive processes that allow us to make sense of sensory information. The interplay between sensation and perception, combined with our environment's richness, creates our perception of the world around us.
Visual object recognition is a fundamental process whereby sensory input is linked to representations stored in memory. The recognition process is complex and there isn’t a definitive single theory that accounts for how we recognize objects; instead, several theories have been proposed to explain this phenomenon.
Template Matching
Proposes that the brain compares incoming sensory information to templates stored in memory, looking for a precise match.
Structural-Description Theories
Objects are represented abstractly in terms of their parts and the spatial relations among those parts. Recognition involves creating a structural description of the input and comparing it with existing memory representations.
Feature Analysis/Detection Theories
Focus on the identification of distinct features within a visual input and the comparison of these features with stored descriptions in memory.
Recognition-by-Components
Developed by Irving Biederman, this theory suggests that objects are recognized by the geons (geometric icons) that make them up.
View-Based Theories
These theories assert that object recognition is dependent on specific views or perspectives of an object, with multiple angles stored in memory for comparison.
Examples illustrate how different templates may correlate with various objects. For instance:
A strong correlation (100%) means an exact match with stored templates, while a weak correlation (30%) indicates a lesser similarity.
Recognition involves forming a structural description and comparing it with memory to identify the possible parts of the object and how they can be recognized.
Different theories propose different sets of parts that can complicate recognition.
This approach analyzes incoming visual images by breaking them down into features, which are then compared with stored descriptions. For example, components like vertical and horizontal lines can help define objects.
This is a renowned structural description theory that suggests we identify objects based on their components known as geons. Geons are simple geometric shapes that serve as the building blocks of objects.
Some commonly identified geons include:
Wedges
Bricks
Cubes
Cylinders
ConesThese shapes possess significant properties that aid in object recognition.
Viewpoint Invariance
Geons remain recognizable from various angles and are sturdy against visual noise.
Robustness to Occlusion
Geons can still be recognized even when partially obscured. For instance, concave regions are critical cues for identifying an object.
Discriminability
This is associated with nonaccidental properties that remain consistent despite changes in the viewpoint. Examples include specific edges, vertices, and parallel lines that help in object identification.
Research comparing nonaccidental properties in humans and pigeons reveals that structural cueing plays a critical role across different species, showing the biological basis of visual recognition capabilities.
Detection of Nonaccidental Properties
Edge Extraction
Determination of Components
Parsing at Regions of Concavity
Matching of Components to Object Representations
Object IdentificationThese stages highlight the cognitive processes involved in perceiving and identifying objects.
Structural description theories may face challenges, such as instances where the same object's representation differs dramatically depending on the viewpoint (e.g., a book vs. a cigar box).
While geons are significant, recognition can also be highly viewpoint-dependent. It’s suggested that the brain may store only a few specific views of an object and utilizes mental rotation to comprehend them from different angles.
The example provided by Yanagi illustrates performance in categorizing objects based on specific views and size comparisons.
Visual recognition is enshrined in the debate between structural-description theories, like recognition-by-components, and view-based theories. Each approach sheds light on different aspects of perception, and ongoing discourse continues to explore newer models that integrate ecological and constructivist perspectives. This discourse emphasizes the rich details within stimuli and the capacity for learning from diverse exposures, aided by complex neural network models.