Processing affect
Human Nest
Soothing perinatal experiences: No separation from the mother; in indigenous tribes, infants are strapped to the mother's back or breast. This practice lasts for extended periods.
Breastfeeding: In these tribes, breastfeeding can last 2–5 years, which is much longer compared to Western cultures.
Affectionate Touch and Responsivity: Constant interaction and responsiveness between mother and child.
Free Play and Social Embeddedness: Play without restrictions enhances social bonding and development.
Empathy: Has deep evolutionary roots (Darwin, 1871). Freud (1938) noted that infants are empathic from birth. Ferreira (1961) described this as an intuitive, almost supernatural ability.
Intersubjective Infant Experience
Infants are highly receptive to stimuli from the social world. They show "social fittedness" from early on, for example, distinguishing their mother's breast milk by scent (Macfarlane, 1975).
Proto-conversations: Dyadic, emotionally charged exchanges with caregivers (Trevarthen, 1979).
Parental Brain and Social Fittedness
Parental Brain: The caregiving brain adapts to promote children’s social development (Feldman, 2015).
Synchrony: Synchronised interactions between parent and child have long-term developmental effects (Leckman & March, 2011).
Affective Communication
Infants engage in early forms of communication through face-to-face interactions.
Mirror-role of mother: The mother's face reflects the infant’s emotions (Winnicott, 1971).
Synchrony
A universal drive: Synchronisation between individuals and even atoms (Strogatz, 2004).
Parent-child synchrony regulates physiological and emotional processes, leading to long-term developmental outcomes (Feldman, 2007).
Synchrony’s effects: Blurs boundaries between self and other (Paladino et al., 2010), increases similarity and closeness (Rabinowitch & Knafo-Noam, 2015), and promotes prosocial behavior in infants (Cirelli et al., 2014).
Mother-Infant Attunement
(The emotional connection and responsiveness such as synchrony)
Best attunement rates: Occur 28-34% of the time (Tronick & Beeghly, 2011).
Misattunement: Requires rapid repair within 3-5 seconds (Reck et al., 2004). Delayed reparation leads to heightened cortisol (stress) reactivity in infants and attempts at self-soothing when the situation is too overwhelming.
Co-regulation and Neural Development
The neocortex gradually develops, allowing better emotional regulation over time. Until then, the parent acts as the external regulator for the child (Hariri et al., 2000).
Maternal buffering: The presence of a mother lowers amygdala reactivity in children but not adolescents (Gee et al., 2014).
Emotion Regulation and Maternal Presence
Tasks like emotional face recognition improve in children when seated next to their mother, particularly for children with stronger maternal buffering effects (Feldman et al., 1999).
Affective Communication and Development
Critical areas influenced by parent-child affective exchanges:
Physiology and Emotion Regulation (Feldman et al., 1999)
Attachment (Isabella et al., 1989; Stern et al., 1998)
Social Engagement (Feldman, 2007)
Self-formation (Emde, 1983; Tronick & Beeghly, 2011)
Affective Communication and Emotional Development
Winnicott (1971):
Describes the role of the mother as a mirror for the baby. The baby sees a reflection of themselves in the mother’s face, which is crucial for their emotional development.
Healthy Emotional Development:
Depends on coordinated patterns of affective communication between caregiver and infant.
Atypical Communication:
Modern caregiving practices differ significantly from those historically followed by humans, which can affect emotional development (Hewlett & Lamb, 2005).
2. Disrupted Communication
Sensitivity to Timing:
Infants are highly sensitive to the timing of interactions. Studies show that infants can detect even brief lags in communication (Henning & Striano, 2011).
Depressed Mothers:
Infants of mothers with depression often experience disruptions, such as frequent breaks in eye contact or extended pauses in interactions (Feldman, 2007; Zlochower & Cohn, 1996).
3. Parenting Practices and Maternal Depression
Postpartum Depression:
Maternal depressive symptoms at 2-4 months postpartum are associated with lower odds of engaging in healthy parenting practices such as breastfeeding, social interactions, and routines (McLearn et al., 2006).
Reduced Breastfeeding:
Studies suggest that depressive symptoms can lower the likelihood of breastfeeding and engaging in social activities like playing or showing books to the infant (Pope & Mazmanian, 2016).
4. Neural Activity in Depressed Mothers
Response to Infant Cry:
Depressed mothers show reduced neural activation in response to their infants’ cries, indicating emotional disengagement (Laurent & Ablow, 2012).
5. Impact on Infant Behavior and Development
Infant Temperament:
Infant temperament can predict the development of parental depression (Murray et al., 1996).
Emotional Synchrony:
Infants of depressed mothers show lower emotional synchrony, reduced touch synchrony, and greater negative emotionality (Granat et al., 2017; Feldman et al., 2009).
Cortisol Reactivity:
These infants also exhibit increased cortisol reactivity, a marker of stress response, contributing to their emotional difficulties.
6. Arousal Regulation in Infants
Need for Co-modulation:
Infants depend on caregivers to regulate their arousal and raw emotional experiences. When caregivers fail to engage, infants may experience states of hyperarousal or immobility (Schore, 2012; Field, 2010).
Alarm States: Disruptions in affective regulation can trigger fight-or-flight or freeze responses in infants, affecting long-term emotional regulation.
7. Defensive Adaptation and Emotional Development
Relational Disruptions:
Infants of emotionally unavailable caregivers adapt by becoming emotionally defensive, which can result in emotional disorganization and a fragile self-concept (Tronick & Beeghly, 2011; van IJzendoorn et al., 1999).
Winnicott (1954):
Describes this disruption as a pathological seduction of the mind away from the body's needs, leading to a disconnection between psyche and soma.
8. Self-Recognition and Attachment
Mirror Self-Recognition:
Research indicates cultural differences in self-recognition and the development of autonomous vs. related selves (Ross et al., 2017).
Attachment and Self-Recognition:
Securely attached infants are more likely to recognize themselves in mirrors compared to those with attachment insecurities (Lewis et al., 1985).
9. Changes in Adult Attachment Styles
Longitudinal Meta-analysis:
Changes in attachment styles among American college students over time show shifts in attachment dynamics, possibly reflecting broader societal or developmental trends (Konrath et al., 2014).
Human Nest
Soothing perinatal experiences: No separation from the mother; in indigenous tribes, infants are strapped to the mother's back or breast. This practice lasts for extended periods.
Breastfeeding: In these tribes, breastfeeding can last 2–5 years, which is much longer compared to Western cultures.
Affectionate Touch and Responsivity: Constant interaction and responsiveness between mother and child.
Free Play and Social Embeddedness: Play without restrictions enhances social bonding and development.
Empathy: Has deep evolutionary roots (Darwin, 1871). Freud (1938) noted that infants are empathic from birth. Ferreira (1961) described this as an intuitive, almost supernatural ability.
Intersubjective Infant Experience
Infants are highly receptive to stimuli from the social world. They show "social fittedness" from early on, for example, distinguishing their mother's breast milk by scent (Macfarlane, 1975).
Proto-conversations: Dyadic, emotionally charged exchanges with caregivers (Trevarthen, 1979).
Parental Brain and Social Fittedness
Parental Brain: The caregiving brain adapts to promote children’s social development (Feldman, 2015).
Synchrony: Synchronised interactions between parent and child have long-term developmental effects (Leckman & March, 2011).
Affective Communication
Infants engage in early forms of communication through face-to-face interactions.
Mirror-role of mother: The mother's face reflects the infant’s emotions (Winnicott, 1971).
Synchrony
A universal drive: Synchronisation between individuals and even atoms (Strogatz, 2004).
Parent-child synchrony regulates physiological and emotional processes, leading to long-term developmental outcomes (Feldman, 2007).
Synchrony’s effects: Blurs boundaries between self and other (Paladino et al., 2010), increases similarity and closeness (Rabinowitch & Knafo-Noam, 2015), and promotes prosocial behavior in infants (Cirelli et al., 2014).
Mother-Infant Attunement
(The emotional connection and responsiveness such as synchrony)
Best attunement rates: Occur 28-34% of the time (Tronick & Beeghly, 2011).
Misattunement: Requires rapid repair within 3-5 seconds (Reck et al., 2004). Delayed reparation leads to heightened cortisol (stress) reactivity in infants and attempts at self-soothing when the situation is too overwhelming.
Co-regulation and Neural Development
The neocortex gradually develops, allowing better emotional regulation over time. Until then, the parent acts as the external regulator for the child (Hariri et al., 2000).
Maternal buffering: The presence of a mother lowers amygdala reactivity in children but not adolescents (Gee et al., 2014).
Emotion Regulation and Maternal Presence
Tasks like emotional face recognition improve in children when seated next to their mother, particularly for children with stronger maternal buffering effects (Feldman et al., 1999).
Affective Communication and Development
Critical areas influenced by parent-child affective exchanges:
Physiology and Emotion Regulation (Feldman et al., 1999)
Attachment (Isabella et al., 1989; Stern et al., 1998)
Social Engagement (Feldman, 2007)
Self-formation (Emde, 1983; Tronick & Beeghly, 2011)
Affective Communication and Emotional Development
Winnicott (1971):
Describes the role of the mother as a mirror for the baby. The baby sees a reflection of themselves in the mother’s face, which is crucial for their emotional development.
Healthy Emotional Development:
Depends on coordinated patterns of affective communication between caregiver and infant.
Atypical Communication:
Modern caregiving practices differ significantly from those historically followed by humans, which can affect emotional development (Hewlett & Lamb, 2005).
2. Disrupted Communication
Sensitivity to Timing:
Infants are highly sensitive to the timing of interactions. Studies show that infants can detect even brief lags in communication (Henning & Striano, 2011).
Depressed Mothers:
Infants of mothers with depression often experience disruptions, such as frequent breaks in eye contact or extended pauses in interactions (Feldman, 2007; Zlochower & Cohn, 1996).
3. Parenting Practices and Maternal Depression
Postpartum Depression:
Maternal depressive symptoms at 2-4 months postpartum are associated with lower odds of engaging in healthy parenting practices such as breastfeeding, social interactions, and routines (McLearn et al., 2006).
Reduced Breastfeeding:
Studies suggest that depressive symptoms can lower the likelihood of breastfeeding and engaging in social activities like playing or showing books to the infant (Pope & Mazmanian, 2016).
4. Neural Activity in Depressed Mothers
Response to Infant Cry:
Depressed mothers show reduced neural activation in response to their infants’ cries, indicating emotional disengagement (Laurent & Ablow, 2012).
5. Impact on Infant Behavior and Development
Infant Temperament:
Infant temperament can predict the development of parental depression (Murray et al., 1996).
Emotional Synchrony:
Infants of depressed mothers show lower emotional synchrony, reduced touch synchrony, and greater negative emotionality (Granat et al., 2017; Feldman et al., 2009).
Cortisol Reactivity:
These infants also exhibit increased cortisol reactivity, a marker of stress response, contributing to their emotional difficulties.
6. Arousal Regulation in Infants
Need for Co-modulation:
Infants depend on caregivers to regulate their arousal and raw emotional experiences. When caregivers fail to engage, infants may experience states of hyperarousal or immobility (Schore, 2012; Field, 2010).
Alarm States: Disruptions in affective regulation can trigger fight-or-flight or freeze responses in infants, affecting long-term emotional regulation.
7. Defensive Adaptation and Emotional Development
Relational Disruptions:
Infants of emotionally unavailable caregivers adapt by becoming emotionally defensive, which can result in emotional disorganization and a fragile self-concept (Tronick & Beeghly, 2011; van IJzendoorn et al., 1999).
Winnicott (1954):
Describes this disruption as a pathological seduction of the mind away from the body's needs, leading to a disconnection between psyche and soma.
8. Self-Recognition and Attachment
Mirror Self-Recognition:
Research indicates cultural differences in self-recognition and the development of autonomous vs. related selves (Ross et al., 2017).
Attachment and Self-Recognition:
Securely attached infants are more likely to recognize themselves in mirrors compared to those with attachment insecurities (Lewis et al., 1985).
9. Changes in Adult Attachment Styles
Longitudinal Meta-analysis:
Changes in attachment styles among American college students over time show shifts in attachment dynamics, possibly reflecting broader societal or developmental trends (Konrath et al., 2014).