Piaget proposed six substages within the sensorimotor stage that detail how infants learn and interact with the world.
Birth to 1 month.
Infants use reflexes such as sucking and grasping as initial ways to explore the world.
1 to 4 months.
Infants repeat pleasurable actions centered on their own body, like thumb-sucking, which leads to new discoveries.
4 to 8 months.
Infants start to interact intentionally with the environment, repeating actions that cause interesting results in the external world.
8 to 12 months.
Infants combine actions to achieve goals, demonstrating problem-solving and intentional behavior.
12 to 18 months.
Infants experiment with new actions to see the outcomes, showing curiosity through trial-and-error approaches.
18 to 24 months.
Infants can solve problems mentally and think about their actions before executing them, reflecting symbolic thought.
For two-month-old infants, "out of sight" equates to "out of mind" because they have not yet developed the concept of object permanence.
Object permanence is the understanding that objects continue to exist even when they cannot be seen, developed around 8-12 months through repeated interactions.
Critics suggest that Piaget may have underestimated infants' capabilities and that cognitive development may be more continuous than his stage theory suggests.
Some argue that clear signs of object permanence can be observed earlier than Piaget proposed.
Memory in Infants: Infants show memory capabilities, such as recognizing familiar stimuli and recalling past experiences.
Imitation: Infants can imitate actions they have observed, which suggests cognitive engagement and social learning.
Researchers begin measuring intelligence in infants typically around 6-12 months using developmental scales like the Bayley Scales of Infant Development.
Various measures exist, including language ability, problem-solving skills, and social interactions.
Visual Recognition Memory: This ability allows infants to recognize previously seen objects or faces, aiding in social interaction and bonding.
Cooing: Early vocalization sounds (around 2-4 months) characterized by frequent consonant-vowel combinations.
Babbling: More complex sound production (around 4-6 months) involving repetitive consonant-vowel combinations.
Receptive Vocabulary: Understanding spoken language versus Expressive Vocabulary: the actual words used in speech. Receptive generally develops faster than expressive.
General Nominals: Words that refer to objects or substances in a category (e.g., "dog," "cat").
Specific Nominals: Words that refer to individual items or entities (e.g., "Fido," "Whiskers").
Both types help children categorize and communicate but differ in specificity and function.
Referential Language Style: Focused on naming objects and actions, common in early language development.
Expressive Language Style: Emphasizes feelings and social relationships, including more personal and emotional speech.
Children may gravitate toward one style based on environmental influences.
Mean Length of Utterance (MLU): Measures the average length of spoken sentences in morphemes, reflecting linguistic complexity.
Morphemes: The smallest grammatical units in language.
Holophrases: Single words that convey complex ideas or emotions at early stages of development.
Modeling plays a significant role, where children learn language by imitating adults and peers around them, highlighting the nature versus nurture debate.
Skinner suggested that language acquisition occurs through shaping, where reinforcement is used to reward correct language use in children, shaping their ability to speak.
Chomsky argued that environmental factors interact with innate capacities in the brain to influence language development, emphasizing the limits of behaviorist theories.
Parts of the brain such as Broca’s area (speech production) and Wernicke’s area (language comprehension) are crucial for language development.
Learning language is most effective during sensitive periods due to heightened neuroplasticity.
Motherese: The slow, exaggerated speech and high pitch often used by caregivers when talking to infants.
This form of speech aids language development by making sounds and words more distinct and engaging for infants.