Commonly defined as the scientific study of mind, brain, and behaviour
Cognitive, neuro, biological, developmental, social, organisation, evolutionary
Science is grounded in observations
data is needed to confirm
Science is cumulative
Science is self-correcting
Science achieves explanation and understanding
The scientific method implies incremental refinement
our knowledge progressively becomes a closer approximation to truth/reality
observation/data → explanation/theory → prediction/hypothesis
Real scientific inference requires exercising judgement
How trustworthy are the data upon which influences are based? (Judgements about the quality of the data)
Is the theoretical explanation a general one, or is it limited to this specific instance? (Judgements about the adequacy of theory)
Does the experiment show the effect that the researcher thinks it does or if the effect arises for the reasons the experimenter thinks it does? (Judgements about the alternative explanations)
Is the evidence reliable?
Is the measures a valid indicator of the construct?
Does repeating the experiment achieve the same results
Ideally, measures should be both reliable and valid
both are required for making legitimate inferences
Reliability refers to how “repeatable” or consistent a measure is
if you were to asses the same construct in the same way, using the same method of measurement, do you tend to get the same results?
Validity refers to the degree to which a measure assesses the thing it is purposed to assess
Is the construct you seek to measure actually related to the measurement?
Racial Hierarchy = general consensus was that whites were on top and blacks on the bottom—but this had no supporting data
Morton came up with measuring cranical capacity to find out the intelligence
Bigger skull→bigger brain=more intelligent
Problem = cranial capacity does not really relate to intelligence, but it does relate to overall body size
Morton’s measure conflated with body size
If the measure is not a valid reflection of the construct of interest, neither are the inferences based on the measure
Is the theory general?
A scientific explanation should apply to more than just one specific case
Can the theory be tested?
Does the theory predict novel observations?
Are there results that would falsify the theory?
Is the theory parsimonious?
A parsimonious theory provides the simplest possible explanation that suffices to explain all relevant observations
Principle of Ockham’s Razor
Can we rule out alternative explanations?
If multiple explanations can explain the data is there a way to distinguish them?
Correlation vs Causation
Because two variables are related to each other does not mean that one causes the other
Allows us to put a numerical value on a measurement
Quantifies our uncertainty
Permits objective measurement by others
Most importantly, Quantitative Measurement allows for comparisons
Of groups of individuals
Of the same individual through time
In most cases, we measure something about a sample of people and seek to form generalised conclusions in the population at large
What is true of the sample/population need not be true of the individual
What is true of the sample need not be trust of another sample (even if both are drawn from the same population)
For example, a study using the big five personality test with 2000 women and 2000 men found that women scored much higher than men in their agreeableness. From these samples it was inferred that women are generally more agreeable than men. This does not mean that all 2000 women were more agreeable than all 2000 men, or that a repeated study of 4000 different people from the same city would demonstrate the same results. However, there were clearly individual women that were more agreeable (according to the test) than individual men in order to get this result. Furthermore, what is true of the sample (women are more agreeable than men), is not true of all women in the area where the sample was taken. However there are obviously individual women who are more agreeable than individual men in the general population area.
Testing the efficacy of a treatment for depression
Can compare a control group (standard treatment) with a new treatment group.
Then, assess post-treatment depression score—is the new treatment effective? (Lower post-depression in new treatment to standard treatment)
Sometimes a study will produce evidence for an effect when there is no true effect to be found
false positive rate (5%)
Sometimes a study will fail to produce evidence for an effect even though there is a true effect to find
Science is defined as any discipline that makes use of the so-called scientific method.
This is an approach to understanding the world through cycles of developing and testing theories.
Psychology is a scientific discipline through its reliance on the scientific method for refining our understanding of mind, brain, and behaviour.
The scientific method is the iterative means by which scientific knowledge is amassed tested and refined.
It describes the relationship between three components:
Observations/data
data are facts about the world; they are phenomena that a scientific discipline seeks to explain
Explanations or theories
theories are statements that seek to organise data coherently
the structure of theories tends to be shaped by data
a theory explains how different observations relate to one another and, more importantly, explains why the facts are the way that they are
Predictions or hypotheses
logical implications that follow from a theory
predictive statements that can be shown to be consistent or inconsistent with future observations
Theories must have two properties to be considered scientific:
Testable predictions — does the theory generate hypotheses that can be evaluated against data?
Falsifiability — the predictions must allow for the theory to be shown to be false. That is, the theory must be able to make a prediction that can, in principle, be shown to be incorrect.
Refining scientific knowledge
If a theoretical prediction is confirmed by the observation, then the theory is provisionally accepted, and another hypotheses is tested
A theory successfully predicting something does not confirm the theory. Rather, a theory that successfully predicts data is simply one that can be kept and tested again
If a theory generates predictions that are shown to be inconsistent with subsequent observations, then the theory can be considered to be falsified.
If it is falsified, the theory can then be adjusted by refining its assumptions and seeing if revisions of the theory can bring it in accord with the data, or, the theory can be rejected outright and abandoned in favour for some other alternative thory
Root metaphor of the blank slate
out behaviours are wholly determined by our environment
Key concepts of History of Reinforcement and Learning
law of effect—behaviours that are rewarded tend to be repeated
rejection of unobservable processes as unscientific
Questions about how contingencies pairing stimuli with reward/punishment affect subsequent behaviour
Root metaphor of computer
inputs are processes and transformed into outputs
Key concepts of mental representations and mental states
focus on unobservable mental processes and their (observable) effets
attention, memory and decision-making
Questions about the mental processes that give rise to behaviour
studied experimentally, often with human participants
Root metaphor of the biological machine
What are the physical bases of thought and behaviour?
Key concepts of evolution, genetics, and physiological functions
Focus on identifying physiological correlates of specific behaviours/cognition
Measurements of brain activity and identification of genetic contributions to behaviours or psychological disorders
Questions about how mental processes are physically realised in the brain and how such functionality evolved
studied experimentally (humans and animals) as well as via case studies
People often seek out information that confirms their beliefs
Occurrence of expected or favoured events are highlighted
Occurrence of unexpected or unfavourable events are minimised
Impose control or structure over the observations we make
The more structure there is, the more confident we can be about the causal status between events
A variety of methods are often used to provide converging evidence for a theory
Specifically set up to support causal inference
Manipulate an independent variable while measuring the effect on another dependent variable
the different ‘levels’ of the IV create different experimental conditions
All other factors are held constant across conditions by either being allowed to vary randomly or by being deliberately equated
If the DV changes significantly across conditions, we may infer that the change was caused by manipulation of the IV
Example = the dependent variable is response time and independent variable is the level of rest
Bias refers to factors that affect the data that are obtained in a study
Can have follow-on effects on conclusions and theoretical inferences
If bias is not detected, conclusions/inferences can be compromised
When the study sample is not representative of the population to which you wish to generalise the study conclusions to
single-blind research can restrict participant knowledge of study aims
double-blind research can restrict experimenter knowledge of participant groups
Bias from participant expectations
placebo effects
hawthorne effect
stereotype threat
demand effects
Bias from experimenter expectations
rosenthal effects (pygmalion/golem)
Defining variables in terms of the ‘operations’ (methods) used to observe/measure/manipulate them
differences in response time are identified with the amount of conflict information that is being processed
Operational definitions are not all equally valid
some measures are more clearly linked to theoretical concepts than others
Paradigm = a typical example or pattern of something; distinct set of concepts or thought patterns, including theories, research methods, postulates, and standards for what constitute legitimate contributions to a field; a framework for understanding and investigation phenomena within a discipline
They determine what standard standard operation—or normal scientific activities—within a discipline looks like
Paradigms define
concepts that are used in theories
research questions that are addressed by a discipline
methodologies used to investigate these questions
Paradigms offer perspective on how phenomena are understood and the means by which once can achieve an understanding of phenomena
Similar concepts include ideologies (liberalism, etc) and world views (atheistic or theistic world view) as they will affect and change your opinions on matters
There are five major paradigms in the history of psychology
clinical practice
psychodynamic
humanist
experimental psychology (adheres to scientific method)
behaviourist
cognitive
biological
Arose partly due to the rejection of Freud’s psychodynamic theories
Behaviourist psychologists encouraged people to consider how the external environment shapes our thoughts and behaviours
Humans, and other animals, can be viewed as blank states whose psychology is determined wholly by the external environment
“product of one’s environment”
John B. Watson, early 1900s
According to Watson, a proper scientific psychology must focus on publicly observable phenomena, such as overt behaviours
Shifted study to relationships between publicly and observable stimuli and their behavioural consequences
BF Skinner
Skinner suggests that internal events, such as thoughts and feelings, could be understood according to the same learning principles that can explain publicly observable behaviours
Although, the external environment was still viewed as the determinant of both observable and unobservable behaviours
Rejects the idea that mind and mental events play any causal roles in human psychology—they are ultimately caused by external factors
Moved away from Behaviourism in the 1950s
Places mental events at the centre of psychological research
Regarded as the current dominant paradigm in psychology
The key assertions that mental events can be studied scientifically and that they play a causal role in determining behaviour
Seeks to understand the processes that “transform” stimuli into behaviours
Most of the research conducted in this paradigm concern itself with identifying cognitive processes that are required to relate changes in stimuli with changes in behaviour
Determining how unobservable processes interact is inherently a difficult task, and so a lot of contemporary work makes use of sophisticated mathematical models that attempt to quantitatively describe how different cognitive processes interact
Does not focus exclusively on abstract functional relationships between cognitive processes
Seeks to explain cognition and behaviour in terms of biological processes
Synergistic with work within cognitive and behaviourist paradigms
fMRI and EEGs allow research to identify neural correlates of cognition and behaviour
Research in the biological paradigm is often more interested in the biological mechanisms that implement or enable cognition, and identifying patterns of neural activity that are associated with specific cognitive activities
Can appear more descriptive than explanatory
~2% of body weight
receives ~20% of blood pumped from the heart
consumes ~20% of body’s energy
100 billion neurons
1,000,000 billion synapses
10^1,000,000 possible circuits
Cerebrum
cerebral hemispheres forebrain
two hemispheres, divided by longitudinal fissures or inter-hemispheric fissure
Cerebral cortex is the outermost surface layer of the cerebrum
Cortext = grey matter
surface of the brain 2-4mm thick
contains the cells bodies of the brain’s neurons
highly folded to maximise surface area (amount of cortex that can fit inside skulls)
white matter underneath the grey matter is all the wiring (axons of the neurons, connecting to the spinal cord and to other areas of the cortex
Cerebellum = hind brain
Brain stem
Executive functions
reasoning, planning, problem solving
inhibitory control
working memory
Motor functions
premotor cortex — motor planning
primary motor cortex — execution
speech production (broca’s area)
Primary somatosensory cortex
perception of touch
Sense of space and locations
gives sense of stable world around us relative to our position
Spatial Attention
directing attention and eye-movements to explore visual world
Linking vision to action
represents spatial location of objects around us for guiding actions
Posterior part of the brain, inferior to parietal lobe
Primary visual cortex
all visual perceptions
Higher visual areas
different regions process shapes, colours, orientation, motion
Primary auditory cortex
perception of sound
Language comprehension (wernicke’s area)
Amygdala
fear and arousal, responds to threat/danger
fear & learning phobias
Hippocampus
learning and memory, forming new episodic memories
damage causes anterograde amnesia (can’t form new memories)
Neuron Connections between the left and right hemispheres
Allows brain communication between hemispheres
Split-brain patients—left & right hemispheres disconnected. The two hemispheres cannot communicate with each other
Railway worker
A iron rod, 1m in length, went through his head in 1848, yet he remained conscious during and after accident
Damaged frontal lobes
Died 12 years later
Suffered a profound change in personality — fitful, irreverent, indulging in profanity, no restraint
In 1861, Paul Broca described a patient who was unable to speak after damage to the left frontal lobe (Broca’s area)
Speech is slow and non-fluent
Difficulty finding appropriate words (anomia)
speech still carries meaning
comprehension is (mostly) unaffected
In 1874, Carl Wernickle suggested that lessions to the left posterior temporal lobe led to deficits in language comprehension
Unable to understand language — deficit in comprehension
Speech is fluent with normal prosody (rhythm, intonation)
Speech has no meaning, nonsense speech
Stimulated the brain with electrical probes while the patients were conscious, during surgery for epilepsy
Published maps of motor and sensory cortices of the human brain
Primary sensory cortex and primary motor cortex
Brain function mapped by electrical stimulation
Brain stimulation leads to sensation or movement (muscle twitch)
Size of area on cortex determines sensitivity or fine motor control
Central Nervous System = brain and spinal cord
Peripheral Nervous System
Somatic nervous system = voluntary, motor, and sensory
Autonomic Nervous System
Involuntary
Heart-rate, respiration, sweating
Stress, arousal, fight-or-flight
2 divisions
Sympathetic Nervous System
emotional arousal, stress, fear
fight or flight response
increases heart-rate, respiration, perspiration, pupils dilate
Parasympathetic Nervous System
Rest and digest
lowers heart rate, respiration
Increases stomach, intestine activity (digestion)
opposes the sympathetic nervous system
Autonomic nervous system functions
Controls heart rate, respiration, regulation of blood pressure, body temperature
Reflex centres for coughing, sneezing, swallowing, vomiting
Severe damage to upper brain (hemispheres and cortex)
If brainstem is not damaged, autonomic nervous system functions can remain
Sometimes normal respiration, control of heart rate, some face and eye movements remain
Patients have NO conscious awareness
Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis (ALS) or Motor Neuron Disease
loss of motor neurons to spinal cord
or brain injury (following accident)
Intact cerebrum and brainstem, but ‘disconnected’ from spinal cord
Normal cognitive function, vision, and hearing, but patients cannot move
May be fully conscious and aware, but totally unresponsive
Hind brain
Sense of balance and co-ordination of complex movement
Motor learning — fine adjustment of movement based on feedbacl
Primary motor cortex activity leads to movement (muscle contraction)
Primary sensory cortex activity leads to sensation
Different parts of motor and sensory cortex map to different parts of the body (homunculus)
Movements planned and ‘programmed’ in the brain before initiation, like a computer program (theory from 1960s)
Brain creates program just before movement OR retrieves program for learnt skilled actions
Brain automatically links sensory events and own actions to infer causality
Sense that my action caused that event
Common to all cells
Contains nucleus and all structures necessary for cell functioning (DNA)
Unique to neurons
Receives signals — input zone
Many per neuron, receives input from many other neurons
Unique to neurons
Sends signals — output from axon hillock at cell body to axon terminals
One per neuron — only one axon for output
Wrapped in myelin for efficient transmission of signals along the axon
Terminal boutons/buttons
Form synapses with other neurons
Secret neurotransmitters to send signals across synapses to other neurons
Brain contains neurons and Glial Cells
Supporting cells for neurons
Produces the myelin sheath that wraps around axons
Supplies nutrients from blood to the neurons
Maintains blood-brain barrier
Brains immune system
Cleans up foreign or toxic substances
Oligodendrocytes form myelin sheath by wrapping around the axon
Essential for efficient communication, for propagation of signals along axon
Multiple Sclerosis involves loss of myelin, disruption of efficient neural communication throughout the body
Join axon terminals of one neuron to dendrites of another neuron for transmission of signals between neurons
Neural signals go one-way
pre-synaptic = from cell body to axon terminal
post-synaptic = from dendrite to cell body
Chemical ‘messenger’
released from pre-synaptic terminal
acts of post-synaptic receptors
Depolarisation of axon terminal (action potential) triggers release of neurotransmitter
Neurotransmitter acts on receptor on post-synaptic neuron to open ion channels and pass signals
chemical signal neuron-to-neuron
Stores neurotransmitter in synaptic terminal
Joins cell membrane wall to release neurotransmitter into synaptic cleft
recycled: neurotransmitter taken back into pre-synaptic terminal is re-packaged into vesicles
Gates on post-synaptic side (neuron dendrite)
Neurotransmitter in syanptic cleft joins with receptor
Activates receptor to open ion channels on post-synaptic neuron
Transmits signal by opening ion channels and changing membrane potential on synaptic neuron
Lock and key — each receptor only binds to a specific type of neurotransmitter
only activate their specific type of receptor
important for drug effects—drugs can act on specific receptors to cause specific effects
Clears neurotransmitter from synaptic cleft back into pre-synaptic terminal
Break down neurotransmitter in synaptic cleft
Both stop neurotransmitter signalling to post-synaptic neuron — closes ion channels (when neurotransmitter is gone) and turns off the signal
loss of dopamine in the basal ganglia deep in the brain
primarily affects movement
treatment with l-dope replaces dopamine in the brain
Act to keep serotonin in the synaptic cleft for longer which increases serotonin signalling
Selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors (prozac, zoloft, lexapro, lovan, cipramil)
Monoamine oxidase inhibitors (Nardil, parnate)
Action potential
Electrical signal pulse travels along the axon
Fixed size — either on or off, signal or no signal
70% of the brain is water
Water surrounds the cells — extra-cellular fluid
Water fills the cells — intra-cellular fluid
Cell membrane forms barrier between extra-cellular and intra-cellular fluid
Sodium (Na+) and Potassium (K+) positively charged ions
Different concentrations outside and inside cell, across cell membrane
Gives difference in electrical charge (potential) across cell membrane
Membrane Potential Definition = difference in the eletrical charge (voltage) between inside and outside cell, across cell membrane wall
Resting Potential Definition = at rest (not during action potential) more positive ions outside than inside the cell gives overall negative potential (voltage) inside compared with outside the cell
Ion channels in cell membrane wall open and close to pass or block movement of ions across cell membrane
Ions move between intra- and extra-cellular fluid
movement of ions changes electrical potential
Important types
Actively pumps Na+ and K+ across cell membrane
Overall pumps positive charge out of cell (3 Na+ out for every 2 K+ in)
Positive change will naturally move towards negative area (opposites attract)
Maintains negative resting membrane potential (approximately -70mV)
Uses energy — about 25% of body total energy (70% of brain energy)
Transmissions of electrical signal along axon
Input from other neurons (via synapses on dendrites) increase membrane potential
If voltage exceeds threshold, triggers action potential
Depolarisation of cell: membrane potential goes back to zero
occurs in less than 0.002 seconds
Repolarisation: membrane potential back to -70mV resting potential
refractory period — more difficult for another action potential to occur
further to threshold to trigger another action potential
Fixed Size and All-or-None principle:
If threshold level is reached, action potential of a fixed sized will occur. The size of the action potential is always the same for that neuron.
All-or-None: Either a full action potential is “fired” (if membrane potential reaches threshold) or there is no action potential. There are no “large” or “small” action potentials.
The strength of the neuron signal is determined by the rate of repeated action potentials
Conduction along axon
Starts at axon hillock: membrane at axon hillock has lowest threshold to trigger action potential
Depolarisation spreads from site of action potential to neighbouring region of cell membrane: causes neighbouring region to pass threshold to trigger action potential
Repolarisation and undershoot (refractory period) prevents action potential going backwards
Voltage dependent ion channel, closed at resting potential
Open when membrane potential reaches threshold voltage
Allows flow of ions across cell membrane
positive ions can flow from outside into the cel (because positive charge will naturally move towards negative area)
Causes depolarisation of cell (voltage less negative = closer to zero)
Different channels open and close at different membrane potentials (voltage dependent)
Depolarisation: Na+ channels open when voltage exceeds threshold
Na+ flows into cell
Less negative potential
Repolarisation: Na+ channels close and K+ channels open after depolarisation
K+ flows out of cell
plus Na/K pump
more negative potential
Neurotransmitter receptors open ion channels when neurotransmitter binds
Different neurotransmitters bind to and open different ion channels (Na+, K+, Cl-) to change membrane potential in different ways
Receptor binding
Can cause depolarisation (less negative)
Can cause hyperpolarisation (more negative)
Receptor Channels — activated by neurotransmitters
Receptor open channels that cause depolarisation
ESPS = excitatory post synaptic potential
Receptor opens channels that cause hyperpolarisation
IPSP = inhibitory post-synaptic potential
further from threshold for action potential
Excitatory and Inhibitory inputs (via dendrites) combine together
changes membrane potential on postsynaptic cell
Graded Potential on postsynaptic cell depends on strength of synapse connection (on dendrite)
strong connection causes large change in membrane potential
weak connection causes small change
Membrane potential at axon hillock depends on sum and timing of inputs through dendrites
If enough excitatory inputs occur together close enough in time, membrane potential will exceed threshold level for action potential
if membrane potential exceeds threshold level (at axon hillock)
triggers action potential, neuron sends signals along its axon
Neuron receives many, many inputs — has only one output
what combination of inputs will cause this neuron to fire and pass on it’s signal
Brain is enormous integrator of information — adapts with learning (billions of neurons with millions of billions of connections)
Imagine this neuron represents memory of your grandmother.
When this neuron fires, you consciously recall your grandmother
Single neuronrecording done almost entirely in animals.
Our main techniques nowadays in humans measuring brain activities with techniques of EEG and MRI.
Led to basically all of our knowledge of function in different parts of the brain up until around the 1950s.
All came from studies of patients with brain lesions, in particular damage lesions to parts of the brain.
Explain normal brain function = by examining what changes in behaviour or cognition when part of the brain is damaged.
Often examined patients following stroke = strikes caused by blockage of blood supply to part of the brain. That part of the brain doesn't receive oxygen can leave a permanent lesion or damage to that part of the brain or in people following brain injury.
the assumption here is that whatever it is that changes in behaviour or cognition must rely on that part of the brain that's damaged.
When that part of the brain's damaged, a particular behaviour function is lost.
Most accurate for localisation and timing of brain activity, measuring brain activity of the brain, is with single neuron recording.
Using an electrode implant or positioned directly into the brain to measure action potentials firing from individual neurons.
This is done almost exclusively in animals, often in rats or cats.
Although there are more and more studies appearing nowadays using this technique in human volunteers during brain surgery.
determines what it is that that individual neuron is encoding or detecting when it sends its signal off down its axon, communicating to the rest of the brain.
The only signal a neuron can send out is an "all or nothing" fixed size action potential.
what is it that causes that neuron to fire? what kind of stimulus or action or thought or memory does firing of that neurons represent? What's its signalling out to the rest of the brain?
it's highly invasive.
been studied extensively in vision (visual cortex).
vision works by saying what it is that individual neurons encode as they pass their information from very early areas to later areas in the brain, encoding more complex features.
Across those neurons is encoded the whole representation of our visual space.
Hubel and Wiesel. their work through the 1950s and '60s.
Reading out from firing of the motor cortex on this homunculus to try to decode plans, intentions for movement, feed into a computer that can then control a robot. And this is maybe a way of developing neuro prosthetics, prosthetic arms, that patients can control directly from their brain activity.
electroencephalography,
Measures the summed activity from action potentials from the many, many thousands of neurons in the cortex. Basically underneath the locations of electrode sensors that are placed on the scope.
Fit a cap to the person's head with maybe 64 electrodes senses and we're measuring the summed electrical activity coming from action potentials around the area underneath each electrode sensor.
This oscillatory activity of the brain that we measure (the brain waves) is actually the best technique that we have for assessing a person's level of, say, sleep, alertness, and arousal.
The waveforms, the frequency of the oscillations, we see an EEG vary characteristic ways depending on a person's level of alertness or sleep.
Alpha activity.
There's oscillations in activity, these waves of about eight to 12 hertz. So 8-12 peaks, wave, waves per second we call alpha activity.
And it's really considered that kind of idling activity of the brain.
And these alpha ways become extremely large when people are relaxed and sleepy, particularly when they close their eyes.
And the alpha waves gets suppressed when a person's eyes open, alert, particularly engaged in sort of highly demanding cognitive tasks.
So it gives us a measure overall of the person's alertness and cognitive load.
Because EEG is also used clinically for monitoring patients with epilepsy, suspected epileptic seizures, will wear the electrode cap over a long period and doctors will be looking for signs of abnormal spiking electrical activity representing seizure activity in the brain.
In our research EEG, we're typically interested in something else called event related potentials. We do that by presenting a particular stimulus. And then taking short windows or epochs of the EEG activity following that stimulus. And usually will average together many, many trials in response to that stimulus. And what we'll see is different peaks of activity that always occur at the same time point following presentation of that stimulus. So they really represent different stages of information processing in the brain as neurons fire their signal and passing from one level to another level in the brain, there's a whole series of information processing steps.
Activity of neurons in the brain actually generates small electrical currents that we can detect and measure on the surface of the head by electroencephalography, or EEG.
This is a common technique used for measuring brain activity as it changes over time in tasks associated with our perception, cognition, decision-making, and planning actions.
The participant is seated comfortably in front of a computer monitor and keyboard so that they can perform computerized tasks while we measure their brain activity during the task.
The EEG brain activity is measured from electrode sensors that are fitted in an elastic cap on the participant’s head. In this case, the cap contains sixty-four EEG sensors that fit across the person’s entire scalp.
The electrode sensors need to connect with the skin to detect the very small electrical currents that come from brain activity, and so a conductive electrolyte gel is used for each electrode sensor. The electrode sensors are then fitted into the cap and connect with the scalp via the electrolyte gel.
The small electrical signal changes, representing brain activity, that are picked up by the EEG sensors, are monitored in real time. Each line or trace on the screen represents the activity recorded by a single electrode sensor.
Each individual trace shows the sum of the electrical activity of hundreds of thousands of neurons within the region underneath the location of each sensor.
What we see as these rhythmic oscillations of brain activity, or brain-waves, are quite difficult to interpret here and represent the fluctuating activity of the brain going on over time. We do see some changes in the frequency or rate of these oscillation waves with different levels of alertness or arousal, or with increasing cognitive load, but generally in our experiments we are more interested in changes in brain activity that underlie a particular behavioural or cognitive task.
So, once the EEG has started recording, the participant will complete an experiment task and we will record brain activity associated with that task.
In this experiment, the participant is watching a field of moving dots while listening tosimple auditory tones being played. After a delay, the dots begin gradually moving in the same direction, and the participant has to indicate which direction as soon as they can perceive it. What we are actually measuring, though, is the brain activity in response to the auditory tones that the participant is not attending to. We actually see from the EEG activity, that the brain is still tracking and anticipating presentation of the tones automatically, even when the participants are not aware of them and their entire focus is on the moving dots. We can see this in EEG by looking specifically at the brain activity around the time that each auditory tone is presented. The time of each tone is tagged in the EEG recording for later offline analysis.
To get a clear measure of the changing brain activity in response to the auditory tone, we need to record many instances of activity around the time of the tone – usually with over one hundred trials that we then average together.
Once we have collected all of the EEG data in the lab, we then analyse offline by computer. We cut out the segments of the EEG data, called epochs, around every instance when a tone was presented. These epochs are all precisely aligned in time to the exact moment when a tone occurred and, by averaging the epochs together, we can see the precise changes in brain activity over time associated with the brain’s processing of the auditory tone.
These are called “Event Related Potentials” – showing the brain activity related to a particular event.
The event related potentials show us different stages of information processing in the brain, from the earliest stages of sensory perception to later and higher-order stages of cognitive and decision-making processes.
In this case, we can see a series of peaks that represent these different stages of information processing in the brain.
The line at time zero here shows the onset of the auditory tone, and the first peaks here occur around 100 ms following the tone onset and represent early stages of sensory processing in the auditory cortex. Later components, such as the large positive wave around 300 ms after the tone reflect higher-order cognitive processing, such evaluating the tone for relevance or comparing or storing in working memory.
We can see the level of activity over the entire head at each time point, known as a topography map. This map gives us an idea of which areas of the brain were most active over different stages of processing of the auditory tone. This is the limitation of EEG and event-related potentials – while we can see broadly where activity was greatest in the brain over different stages of information processing, it is very difficult to localise precisely where in the brain those processes are occurring.
So Event-Related Potentials, recorded from EEG, give us a very powerful technique to examine the timing of stimulus or information processing in the brain. We can then track the different brain processes that occur through stages of sensory perception, stimulus evaluation, decision-making, and selecting appropriate responses.
studies will often look at particular stages or processes that we think occur in the brain for particular kinds of stimuli.
One well-known peak and component of information processing is for faces.
That in part of the visual system, there seems to be an area that's quite specialised for our ability to process, to recognise people's identity from their faces, the face processing.
And in EEG, we see a large negative peak at about 170 milliseconds after faces presented. And it's something quite specific to faces and represents this activity in this area that's for face recognition.
very good for showing us the precise time of information processing in the brain.
They do provide us a direct measure of electrical activity does come from neuronal firing. Not individual neurons but summed activity from many, many neurons.
And the main problem with event-related potentials though, is it's difficult to accurately localise exactly where in the brain that activity is coming from.
So we say that have relatively poor spatial resolution. That is, we measure activity across the whole surface of the scalp. And we get these maps that can show broadly where on the head the activity's coming from.
But it's hard to map that exactly to a precise location in the brain. So for localisation of brain activity, we turn to functional brain imaging methods.
Techniques started in the 1980s with a technical PET, Positron Emission Tomography, that involved injecting radioactive labeled water, often water or glucose, into the bloodstream and map the location of the blood flow through the brain via this radioactive label.
Nowadays, these techniques are still used and particular chemicals are made to attach sort of radioactive labels to particular neurotransmitters and so we can actually plot out density concentration of neurotransmitters of receptors in the brain.
For just straight brain activations, in what parts of the brain are active with particular tasks, now we use exclusively functional MRI measuring, still measuring changes in blood flow, but related to changes in blood oxygen level.
Anyone who's gone to an MRI scanner clinically will often have done that, to have these beautiful pictures of body's anatomy.
The little 'f' in front for fMRI, functional MRI uses the same machine, but quite a different technique. And all it gives us is a statistical map of where there's a change in blood oxygen level in the brain with a particular task.
As a statistical map we make into colours and we just put it on top of the beautiful anatomical picture of the brain. But the fMRI is just these coloured activation spots, show statistically where there's a change in activation with a particular task.
Functional MRI is actually detecting changes in blood oxygen level. And it's important we know neurons, when they become more active, they consume a lot of energy and they need oxygen, oxygen for energy. And so active neurons are using oxygen.
When we see an increase in brain activity, we actually see an increase in blood flow and more oxygen delivered to that area. And that gives us our increase in fMRI signal.
There's much research on fMRI localising function to particular parts of the brain. But that's basically how the technique works.
Really excellent for our ability to localise where and where in the brain activity changes with a particular task. The problems are, well, it's a relatively indirect = a bit slow and delayed relative to the electrical activity of the neurons themselves.
It is also a very expensive technique.
Functional MRI allows us to localise activity associated with different functions, such as different aspects of attention, cognition, decision-making, or emotion, to specific parts of the brain.
The MRI scanner uses a very strong super-conducting magnet that is always on. It’s extremely important that people do not have any metal on them or inside their body when they go into the scanner. All participants go through a short interview screening procedure first to make sure it is safe for them to go into the scanner.
For the MRI scan, the participant lies on the scanner bed that then goes inside the large outer magnet. The MRI scanner makes a lot of noise during scans and so the participant wears headphones, which allow us to communicate with the participant while they are in the scanner.
The participant lies down with their head in a head coil, which is what captures the images of their brain. They also use a button-response pad which they hold inside the scanner, and so they can respond to tasks in the scanner by pressing buttons on the response pad.
Then rest of the head coil is fitted. There is a mirror on the top of the head coil, in front of the participants’ eyes, and they can watch a computer display screen that is outside the tunnel of the magnet through this mirror on the head coil.
So, for the scan, the person is lying on their back inside the coil of the magnet, they are viewing stimuli on screen via the mirror on the head coil, wearing headphones for the noise, and responding to tasks by pressing buttons on the response-pad by their side.
For functional MRI, now we add a task for the participant to perform inside the scanner, so that we can measure their brain activity to investigate brain function associated with the task.
With functional MRI, we actually measure changes in the oxygen level in blood that happen as brain activity increases. As neurons in the brain increase in activity, firing more rapidly, they consume more energy. Energy in the brain is from glucose and oxygen, which are carried in the blood, so areas of the brain with increased activity actually receive an increased supply of oxygenated blood.
It is this increase in blood oxygen level to active areas of the brain that we measure with fMRI.
Once we have collected all this image data, we analyse later by examining and quantifying the level of MRI signal change, representing increased blood oxygen level, or brain activity, in different regions of the brain.
Nowadays, we can do functional MRI scans with far higher resolution and have very precise localisation of activity within specific regions of the brain, even down to the level of specific layers within the cortex.
We also have far more sophisticated computational techniques, including machine learning methods, that allow us to start to make inferences about the type of information that is encoded in activity that we see in the brain.
Brain plasticity really is how the brain changes with experience and learning.
Brain plasticity or neuroplasticity, is the capability of the brain to alter its functional organisation as a result of experience.
And the brain is constantly changing and adapting, altering through its connections, mostly, with all experience and learning throughout all of life. And this is neuroplasticity.
the importance of these connections, we have to go back to what it is that's causing a neuron to fire and pass on its signal through integration of the many, many inputs that that neuron is receiving from many other parts of the brain through its dendrites.
Each individual neuron having many, many inputs, many, many synapse connections from other neurons in the brain coming in through its dendrites; some excitatory, some inhibitory - all signaling different things.
And that neuron is integrating all that information to determine will it fire its signal, whatever it's representing, and pass that signal on in the brain or not.
So it's going to be things from sensory areas, vision, sounds, smells that all match characteristics of your grandma will all feed into this neuron with excitatory signals and push the membrane potential and this neuron higher, closer to threshold until there's enough inputs all at once that match and this neuron fires. And you think, "oh grandma!"
Anything we learn or encode in memory involves physical changes in the brain. That's what we mean by neuroplasticity. That's the capacity of the brain to change its organization with experience and learning. And this happens constantly throughout all of life.
Ramon Y Cajal, who studied the structure and growth of neurons throughout brain development, and he won the Nobel Prize for his neuron theory of the brain.
He noted that neurons do not regenerate and was absolutely correct.
When neurons in the brain are damaged or die, they never regrow or repair.
The brain actually continues to change physically with learning and experience throughout all of life. In particular, we now know that neurons are born throughout all of life through stem cells that occur naturally within the brain.
Stem cells are cells that are undifferentiated and they can become any kind of cell in the body. n the brain, stem cells differentiate into neurons in two areas:
Hippocampus
One critical for learning and memory is the hippocampus.
We know that the hippocampus is essential for memory because removal of the hippocampus leads to severe memory impairment.
However, we still don't fully understand the significance of neurogenesis in the hippocampus for learning.
This is still a very current and active area of research.
Synaptogenesis
More important, though, the main way that we know in which the brain changes with learning is by forming and strengthening new synapse connections through processes of synaptogenesis, new synapse connections are created in the brain or existing connections are strengthened or weakened.
One important figure in the history of neuroscience of learning is Donald Hebb. A simple observation that he made was that pet rats of exactly the same breed and genetics as laboratory rats still tended to be smarter if raised in a household environment as pets than in laboratory housing.
There's actually a whole range of cognitive tasks that we have to test learning and memory abilities in rats, and rats raised in home environments as pets do better in those tasks than do laboratory rats. This actually spurred on a whole field and a well-used paradigm in learning in animal research known as environmental enrichment, where rats raised in enriched conditions actually show much more extensive growth of dendrites and synaptic connections throughout the brain. If the rats are raised in an enriched condition, having lots of opportunity for sensory and motor exploration and stimulation, rather than the standard laboratory housing, this actually leads to more extensive growth of the dendritic tree of cortical neurons and more extensive synapse connections through the brain.
A really important process, is known as long term potentiation LTP and refers to a strengthening of synapse connections through a whole range of chemical and molecular processes.
Synaptic strength changes with learning.
Now, if we look back, considering when a neuron will fire its action potential, depending on the relative weighting or balance of incoming signals, to dendrite through synapse connections, remember, a neuron will be receiving many thousands of inputs through synapse connections, some excitatory, some inhibitory, some strong, some weak, and in the end, for this neuron, what's important is will its membrane potential exceed the threshold level required for it to fire an action potential and pass on its signal or not?
Now, that said, some of the synapse connections coming into dendrites of this neuron, they may release neurotransmitters that are excitatory and cause a slight increase in membrane potential, an excitatory post-synaptic potential. Some of those synapse connections will release a neurotransmitter that's inhibitory and cause a slight decrease in membrane potential, an inhibitory post-synaptic potential, and those potentials will be strong or weak depending on the strength of synapse connection.
That is neurons that share connections together in a chain and fire their action potentials from one neuron on to the next and cause the next neuron to fire, the synapse connection between those neurons will strengthen every time those neurons fire together.
So Hebbian learning really is a process whereby the repeated firing at both pre-synaptic and post-synaptic neurons firing together strengthens that synapse connection between them, so that, in future, activation of the pre-synaptic neuron becomes more likely to cause the post-synaptic neuron to also fire, through the strengthened connection.
So, in this way, the brain learns a lot through associations, through these repeated firings of neurons, firing together and wiring together. And that's actually the basis of much neuroplasticity and learning, that the brain pathways or connections that are used often get strengthened through this process of Hebbian learning.
The primary sensory and primary motor cortices have this homunculus organisation.
Homunculus, where the size of the area on the cortex represents the sensitivity we have in that part of the body, on the sensory side. Or the level of fine motor control we have with that part of the body on the motor side, and these are also things that are learnt and changed with experience.
Study 1
So this is a study of people who played string instruments using their left hand, very frequently on the strings, right hand really just with a bow.
And so through practice with an instrument they're receiving a lot of sensory input and sensory sensation through the fingers of their left hand.
And in this study, the researchers have used a technique measuring brain activity in response to sensation on the index finger and sensation on the fifth finger, the little pinky finger. And measuring the distance representation or those fingers on the surface of the brain, on the cortex.
And the distance between them represents basically the size of the hand area in sensory cortex for the string players left hand. And they find that that hand area is much larger in string players than non string players for the left hand and also larger for the left-hand then for their right hand, which is not having the same sensory input through the strings.
So just that experience over a long period with sensation through the fingertips is causing a expanding of the hand area on the sensory cortex.
Study 2
We look at what happens with brain reorganisation following damage of, following injury to the brain, a lot of recovery of function after brain damage or injury, for example, following a stroke, occurs by reorganisation of the brain.
That is, some areas that are not damaged, taking over function from damaged areas, which again involves a reconnection, a remapping of undamaged areas now to new functions.
So this is a study actually performed in monkeys where they've deliberately caused lesion in part of the monkeys primary motor cortex.
So as a model of what would happen in stroke if someone has a stroke that affects their motor cortex. They'll actually develop the muscle weakness or even loss of movement on one side of their body.
So in this study, the researchers used electrical stimulation to map out all the areas on the motor cortex that connected with the monkey's hand. They stimulate that part of the brain causes muscle contraction in the hand, the hand area on primary motor cortex for the monkeys. And then they made a small area of lesion within that hand area.
As a result, the monkeys had some impaired movement with their hand. They had a deficit caused by damage to that hand area. And as you can see in the top, the top image, is a model where the monkeys had no rehabilitation and that is no movement.
Basically with damage to the hand, they stopped using their hand. And with lack of use, the area of the brain representing the hand actually shrinks and gets smaller. And that's we call maladaptive plasticity.
The lower picture shows what happened with rehabilitation. In this case, the monkeys good hand was restrained and so it was basically forced to use its impaired hand. And with use of that hand, actually the area, the cortex representing the hand started to expand, expand even beyond areas that previously were connected to control of the hand. So other areas taking over function in compensation for damaged areas. And that growth of the handy on the motor cortex associated with improved function for the monkey in its hand movement. So with use particularly rehabilitation motor training was in expanding and other areas taking I have a function from damaged areas.
Study 3
A really dramatic case which shows you how incredible this plasticity can be with brain reorganisation is this really interesting study of blind people reading braille with FMRI.
They actually took blind people who read Braille and sighted people and taught them how to read braille. And they found in the blind people their visual cortex actually takes over function, shows activity, with reading braille.
Braille, of course, has little spots on the dots on the page that are read through the fingertips through sensation.
So should involve sensory cortex, somatosensory cortex but in blind people that primary visual cortex otherwise is lacking input and there's still functional neurons there. And it appears that the brain reorganises and those areas take I have a function for reading, although through fingertips and somatosensation. And this only happened in the blind people.
Explains normal brain function by examining what changes when part of the brain is damaged
Stroke or brain injury in humans
Induced lesions in animals (electrical/chemical)
Assumption: Whatever changes in behaviour/cognition must rely on that part of the brain that is damaged
Place a thin electrode into an animal’s brain (rat, cat, monkey)
record action potentials “firing” from a single neuron
Measure what that neuron encodes or detects
What causes it to fire?
What stimulus / action / thought (?) does it represent?
Best Localisation and Timing of brain function
Directly measuring action potentials from individual neurons
Problems
Highly invasive
electrodes directly into brain
Animals only
(although some studies now measure single neuron firing in humans during brain surgery)
Hubel and Wiesel, 1959
First recordings from visual cortex neurons in cats
Nobel Prize in 1981
Summed activity from action potentials of neurons in the cortex cause electrical activity change on the scalp (skin of the head)
Measure voltage changes from electrodes placed on the scalp
Waveforms vary with brain states:
Sleep and Alertness
Brain activity in EEG shows constant oscillations (waves)
Frequencies of oscillations change with alertness and sleep
Clinical Uses:
Detecting stages of sleep
Monitoring for Epileptic seizures
Brain activity related to a specific event or stimulus
Average together >100 trials of EEG in response to the stimulus
Peaks represents different stages of processing of the stimulus
Example:
Auditory Event-Related Potentials
Activity over time in different parts of the brain auditory pathway for processing sounds
Clinical Use:
Detecting deafness in babies
ERPs can show precise time of information processing in the brain
Direct measure of electrical activity (neuron firing) in the brain
Problems
Difficult to accurately localise activity to specific brain areas
Poor spatial resolution
Measures electrical potential conducted across the scalp
Hard to determine exactly where in the brain this activity comes from
ERPs can show precise time of information processing in the brain
100ms Viewing any stimuli
peak of brain activity 100ms after seeing visual stimulus
Early brain processing of general visual features (brightness, colours, edges)
170ms Viewing faces
peak of brain activity 170ms after seeing face
brain processing for face recognition in visual cortex
PET: Positron Emission Tomography
1980 to late 90’s
Uses radioactive substances injected into bloodstream
Used now to map neurotransmitters or receptors (radioactively labelled “tracers”) in the brain
fMRI: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
1992 to current
Measures change in blood oxygen level
MRI studies brain anatomy.
Functional MRI (fMRI) studies brain function.
fMRI measures changes in blood oxygen level that accompany changes in brain activity
Good localisation of brain activity
Problems
Indirect measure of brain activity
BOLD signal from change in blood oxygen level
Not precise timing of neural activity
very expensive !!
Functional MRI detects change in Blood Oxygen Level (BOLD signal)
Active neurons use Oxygen
Oxygen is carried in blood – delivered to active neurons
Change in Blood Oxygen Level = Change in Brain Activity
Increased Brain Activity → increased Blood Flow (more oxygen delivered) → Increased fMRI signal
Participants viewed checkerboard
Off for 60s
Flashing for 60s
Off for 60s
Flashing for 60s
Increased BOLD signal (increased blood flow = increased brain activity) in visual cortex when flashing
Cannot infer what people are thinking or doing or feeling based on measurement of their brain activity !!!
Reverse Inference = bad!
Good Experimental Design:
Manipulate one factor – independent variable (eg. task or behaviour) and Measure the effect on the dependent variable (eg. brain activity).
Cannot do reverse: Cannot look at brain activity (dependent variable) to determine what the independent variable was (i.e. task people were doing)
How the brain changes with learning
Definition: The capability of the brain to alter its functional organisation as a result of experience.
Neurogenesis and Synaptogenesis
Generation of new neurons and synapses (connections)
Neuron receives many, many inputs – has only one output
What combination of inputs will cause this neuron to “fire” and pass on it’s signal?
Brain is enormous “integrator” of information – adapts with learning (billions of neurons with millions of billions of connections)
When sum of all inputs is high enough, triggers output (cell “fires”)
Imagine this neuron represents memory of your grandmother
When this neuron “fires” you consciously recall your grandmother
What information does this neuron need to receive to “fire” and give conscious recall of your grandmother?
“Grandmother Cells” – all theoretical
Neurons could “represent” (encode or “fire” to) a specific concept, such as your grandmother (Jerry Lettvin, 1969)
Billions of neurons can encode billions of concepts
Memory may be represented by groups of neurons each encoding specific concepts or objects
“Jennifer Aniston cells” found in Hippocampus
Recording from neurons in hippocampus in epilepsy patients
Fire specifically to pictures of Jennifer Aniston
Spreading Activation Model - Theory
Neurons represent a specific concept (eg. Grandmother cells)
Share connections with neurons that represent related concepts
Eg. Fire-engine èRed, Truck, Fire, Siren
Activation (firing) of one neuron leads to spreading activation to related or connected neurons (concepts)
Learning and Memory
Making and strengthening connections between neurons that represent associated concepts
Studied growth of neurons and axons during brain development
Neurons do not regenerate
“In the adult centers the nerve paths are something fixed, ended and immutable. Everything must die, nothing may be regenerated. It is for the science of the future to change, if possible, this harsh decree.”
Cajal was mostly right, but not entirely …
Neurons never regenerate or repair: damaged brain areas never “re-grow” BUT
New neurons constantly “born” throughout life from Neural stem cells
Only two areas in adult brain:
Hippocampus (learning and memory)
Subventricular zone for olfactory bulb
Synaptogenesis
Definition: Generation of new synapses: brain connections
New synapses are constantly formed and strengthened with experience and learning
“Enriched” conditions lead to growth of dendrites and more extensive synaptic connections
Long-Term Potentiation (LTP)
Change in the structure of synapses to give stronger signal from pre-synaptic to post-synaptic neuron
Many mechanisms
Eg. More post-synaptic receptors
focus of cellular/molecular neuroscience research on memory and learning
Excitatory and Inhibitory inputs (via dendrites) sum together
Change membrane potential at axon hillock
Graded Potentials
Graded Potential at axon hillock depends on strength of synapse connection (on dendrite)
Strong stimulus causes large change in membrane potential
Weak stimulus causes small change
“Neurons that fire together wire together”
“When an axon of cell A is near enough to excite cell B and repeatedly or persistently takes part in firing it, some growth process or metabolic change takes place in one or both cells such that A's efficiency, as one of the cells firing B, is increased.” Donald Hebb, 1949.
Repeated firing of pre-synaptic and post-synaptic neuron “firing together” strengthens synaptic connection
Brain “learns” associations through repeated pairings
Strengthens connections between paired stimuli or events
Basis of Neuroplasticity Learning:
Brain pathways (connections) that are used often are strengthened
Primary Sensory cortex and Primary Motor cortex
Size of area on cortex determines sensitivity or fine motor control
Studied sensory cortex in musicians who play stringed instruments
Measured activity for index and little finger sensation for left and right hand
String players had larger area on primary sensory cortex for left hand fingers than non string players
Sensory cortex finger areas expand with use and experience
Lesioned motor cortex in monkeys and used electrical stimulation to map hand area
With no rehabilitation: (no movement) motor cortex area for hand got smaller: maladaptive plasticity
With rehabilitation: (movement training) motor cortex area for hand expanded and movement improved.
After damage, motor cortex can re-organise with use to recover function.
Studied brain activity in blind people while reading Braille
Brain activity in visual cortex while reading Braille
Visual cortex changed only in blind participants. Sighted participants did not show visual cortex activity when reading Braille
Brain areas lacking their normal input can take new functions with use
Popular myth: left hemisphere = analytical, right hemisphere = creative – no basis in reality.
True lateralization: few functions localized to one hemisphere.
Most brain functions require coordination of both hemispheres.
Language and speech: in most people, left hemisphere (Broca’s area).
Tone of voice & prosody: more right hemisphere.
Face perception: stronger in right hemisphere.
Perceptual grouping: right hemisphere tends to see overall patterns.
Motor, sensory, and visual areas:
Left hemisphere → controls/feels right side of body.
Right hemisphere → controls/feels left side.
Visual fields: left side of vision → right hemisphere; right side → left hemisphere.
Most people: left hemisphere for both speech and movement.
Right-handers: ~95% have language in left hemisphere.
Left-handers: ~70% have language in left hemisphere, 30% in right.
Hand dominance ≠ language dominance.
Corpus callosum: main structure allowing info transfer between hemispheres.
Crucial for tasks that need info from both sides (e.g., naming objects seen in left visual field).
Flash images to left or right visual field to control which hemisphere processes them.
Example:
Sun flashed right → left hemisphere sees it, can say “sun”.
Sun flashed left → right hemisphere sees it, info crosses corpus callosum to left hemisphere to name it.
Corpus callosum cut (to treat epilepsy) → no inter-hemispheric communication.
Everyday life: patients function normally.
Lab tasks reveal split functions:
Picture flashed to left field: right hemisphere sees it, but no speech → patient says “nothing”.
But can point or draw with left hand (right hemisphere control) to show what was seen.
Picture flashed to right field: left hemisphere sees it, patient names it easily.
Left hemisphere has verbal language and can describe what it sees.
Right hemisphere can understand language (to some extent) and control left hand to draw/point to what it saw.
No direct speech in right hemisphere.
Right hemisphere can control left hand actions but can’t verbally describe them.
Raises questions about independent “consciousness” in each hemisphere.
HM had hippocampus removed bilaterally to treat epilepsy.
Surgery stopped seizures but caused inability to form new memories (anterograde amnesia).
HM remembered childhood and past events → hippocampus not storage site for old memories.
Hippocampus is essential for encoding new episodic (event-based) memories.
Short-term memory (e.g., holding a phone number) and procedural memory (e.g., mirror drawing) were intact.
HM could learn new motor skills (procedural learning) but not recall learning them.
Short-term memory: brief mental rehearsal.
Long-term memory:
Declarative (explicit):
Episodic: personal experiences, events.
Semantic: facts, knowledge (e.g., Paris is the capital of France).
Procedural (implicit): motor skills (e.g., riding a bike).
HM’s deficit: declarative long-term memory encoding.
Contains place cells → mental maps for navigating familiar environments.
Seizures often originate in medial temporal lobes (near hippocampus).
Epilepsy may damage hippocampus over time due to repeated abnormal firing (excitotoxicity).
Shrinking (atrophy) of hippocampus linked to declining memory performance.
When medication fails, surgical removal of seizure focus (temporal lobe) can control epilepsy.
Surgery can risk memory loss if hippocampus is removed/damaged.
Test verbal memory (left hemisphere, left hippocampus) and visual memory (right hemisphere, right hippocampus).
If both verbal & visual memory are impaired pre-surgery, removing hippocampus risks permanent amnesia.
Temporarily anesthetizes one hemisphere to see if the other can compensate for memory.
If memory disappears during the test, surgery is too risky (risk of amnesia).
Assess memory function pre- and post-surgery.
Follow-up at 3, 6, 12, 24 months.
Offer rehabilitation and strategies to cope with memory deficits.
Important to ensure seizure control to prevent further cognitive decline.
Hippocampus critical for encoding new declarative memories.
Repeated seizures → hippocampal damage → progressive memory loss.
Timely epilepsy treatment is essential to protect memory
Lateralised functions
Some brain functions rely more on one-side of the brain than the other
Left hemisphere
language and speech
Right hemisphere
tone of voice/prosody
face perception
perceptual grouping
Crossing (contralateral) functions
movement, sensation, and visions
Left hemisphere
right body movement and sensation
right side vision
Right Hemisphere
left body movement, sensation, vision
Language is lateralised to the left hemisphere in most people
Language comprehension, speech, reading
Speech production
Language and Hand Dominance
No overall “dominant hemisphere”
Right-handed people
95% have language in Left Hemisphere
(5% in right hemisphere)
Left-handed people (10% of population)
70% have language in Left Hemisphere
(30% in right hemisphere)
Contralateral = opposite side
Ipsilateral = same side
Primary motor and sensory cortex
connect to contralateral (opposite) side of body
right hemisphere to left side of body
left hemisphere to right side of body
Each side of visual space mapped to contralateral visual cortex (opposite side of body)
left side of vision to right hemisphere
right side of vision to left hemisphere
Note: not left eye/right eye
input to each half of retina of each eye is split so that left vision from both eyes foes to the right hemisphere and the right vision from both eyes to left hemisphere
important for 3D depth perception
Connects the left and right hemispheres
Axons of neurons (nerve fibres) crossing to the opposite (contralateral) hemisphere
Neurons send their axons via the corpus callosum to connect with neurons in the opposite hemisphere
Allows transfer of information between the two hemispheres
Vision goes to contralateral hemisphere
In laboratory, can present stimuli very briefly to left or right of screen (on and off before people have time to move their eyes; < 200 ms)
Stimuli go selectively to right or left hemisphere
Language in left hemisphere – can report what is on right side of screen
Stimuli on left of screen è Right Hemisphere
Must cross to Left Hemisphere for language to report what object was
Inter-hemispheric communication across corpus callosum
What about action? How can the person point to the sun? Left and Right hands?
Vision goes to contralateral hemisphere
Left of screen → Right Hemisphere
Movement controlled by contralateral hemisphere
Left hemisphere → Right arm movement
Left Hemisphere for language to report what object was
Left Hemisphere for right hand to point to object
Inter-hemispheric communication across corpus callosum
“Last resort” surgical treatment for very severe epilepsy
Corpus callosum severed to stop seizure activity from spreading to the other hemisphere
Sperry and Gazzaniga studied “split-brain” patients (1960’s)
Led to much knowledge about lateralisation of brain function
Images flashed to left or right of screen - “seen” by only right or left hemisphere
People can reach under screen to touch and feel objects – find them by feel
Right hemisphere can “read” and understand words, but no speech (so no verbal report)
Left hemisphere can tell what it has seen
Right hemisphere can only show it (via the left hand)
Patient cannot say what their left hand is doing !!
Separate “consciousness” in each hemisphere?
Medial temporal lobe
Memory
Forming new episodic memories
Damage causes memory loss (can’t form new memories)
Spatial Navigation
Mental map of familiar environment (Nobel Prize 2014)
“H.M.” had his hippocampus removed to treat epilepsy (in 1953 at age 27)
Cured epilepsy but …caused severe memory loss
Could not form new memories and recall anything from after the time of the surgery.
Would immediately “forget” everything that just happened
Could remember and recall things from before his surgery
Could “mentally rehearse” to remember things for a few seconds
Could learn new skills (but could not remember learning them)
Led to new understanding of memory:
Memory” not one thing, but different components mediated by different parts of the brain
Short-term memory
Lasts several seconds. Eg. remembering phone number long enough to type in to phone
H.M. could “mentally rehearse” to remember things
Long-term memory
Declarative – Conscious recollection (things you can “declare”)
Episodic – Memory of past events or “episodes”, things you’ve seen and done,
eg. what you had for lunch yesterday, what you did on your birthday last year
Semantic – Facts and basic knowledge you can recall and declare
eg. Paris is the capital city of France
Procedural – Not for conscious recall
Skills you have learnt. Eg. how to ride a bike, how to sign your name
H.M. could learn new skills (but not remember having learnt them)
Encoding
Laying down new memories for long-term storage
H.M. could not form new memories
Retrieval
Retrieving memories for conscious recall
H.M. could recall memories from before surgery
Frontal and parietal lobes mediate attention and control behavior.
These lobes are highly evolved in humans.
Top-down: conscious, voluntary control based on goals and experience.
Bottom-up: involuntary attention capture by salient stimuli or unconscious drives.
Example:
Top-down: Finding red keys on a cluttered desk.
Bottom-up: Sudden movement or bright color involuntarily captures attention.
Parietal lobe: crucial for directing spatial attention (often linked with eye movements).
Damage can cause spatial neglect: ignoring one side of space (e.g., left-side neglect after right parietal damage).
William James: attention = selecting one of many possible objects/thoughts.
Acts as a filter to manage limited brain processing capacity.
Selective attention:
Spatial: moving “spotlight” of attention to different locations.
Feature-based: focusing on specific features (e.g., color, shape, sound).
Example:
Where’s Wally: moving attention to find red-and-white stripes.
True multitasking is impossible: we switch attention between tasks.
Example: Not aware of your seat pressure until someone mentions it.
Top-down attention (goal-driven): dorsal network (frontal & parietal regions).
Bottom-up attention (stimulus-driven): ventral network (frontal & parietal).
Key Model:
Corbetta & Schulman model (supported by fMRI & lesion studies).
Damage to parietal cortex (often from stroke) → neglect of contralateral space.
Patients unaware of everything on the affected side.
Frontal lobes: largest lobes, highly evolved in humans.
Crucial for executive functions:
Reasoning, planning, problem-solving.
Inhibitory control (suppressing inappropriate actions/urges).
Everyday control depends on top-down control (conscious goals) over bottom-up urges (unconscious drives).
Example: resisting cake on a diet (top-down inhibits bottom-up desire).
ADHD: impulsivity, distractibility.
OCD: compulsions driven by intrusive obsessions.
Addiction: compulsive behavior, impaired self-control.
Severe frontal lobe damage → loss of behavioral control: impulsive, inappropriate, profane behavior.
Perception (or conscious awareness) and behaviour is a combination of:
bottom-up processes—driven by external stimuli or unconscious states
top-down processes—cognitive control or volitional choice; modulation by prior knowledge and experience
Anterior to the central sulcus
cognitive control of behaviour
executive functions
reasoning, planning, problem-solving
inhibitory control
working memory
motor functions
premotor cortex—motor planning
primary motor cortex—execution
speech (broca’s area)
‘Select’ and prioritise stimuli based on location or features (whatever is relevant for goal)
Moving ‘spotlight’ (location)
Relevant features (colour, shape, etc)
‘Resources’ for attention are limited
We can’t ‘attend’ to all incoming sensory information
We use attention to filter and prioritise sensory information
Selecting and prioritising according to task or goal
Voluntarily shifting visual attention (spotlight) to search
Choosing features for selection, or “focus of attention
Attention “captured” involuntarily by highly salient stimuli
Things that “stand out” or Pop-out,
eg. sudden movement, colours or shapes that stand out from the background capture our attention
Advertisers know how to capture your attention involuntarily, draw your eyes to particular things.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
Network of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortex mediate attention
Different areas for Goal-Directed and Stimulus-Driven attention (top-down and bottom-up)
Based on brain imaging (MRI) studies and patients with brain lesions (spatial neglect)
Lesion (damage) to one hemisphere, frontal or parietal cortex
Most commonly caused by stroke (blockage of blood supply)
Deficit in directing attention to one side of space (side contralateral to brain lesion)
“Ignore” things on one side; Unable to perceive stimuli on side contralateral to brain lesion
Not due to any sensory deficit (i.e. normal vision)
Simultagnosia: Can’t perceive multiple objects simultaneously
Will ignore objects on the neglected side
Frontal-Parietal network necessary for attention to objects and space on the contralateral side
Crucial for control of behaviour
Selection of appropriate actions
Inhibition or suppression of inappropriate actions or usual responses (task-switching)
Many disorders associated with impaired inhibitory control:
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): impulsive behaviours, difficulty preventing distraction to maintain attention on task
Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD): Repetitive compulsive behaviour (washing, cleaning, checking)
Reward Addictions (e.g. gambling, internet: gaming, shopping, pornography): Compulsive behaviours
Commonly used neuropsychological test
Particularly sensitive to attention deficits in children
ADHD: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
Relies on frontal cortex executive control: Ability to focus and maintain attention and inhibit “pre-potent” response (i.e. inhibit the usual rule)
Simple example (lots of other variations)
Letters presented on screen one after the other
Read each letter aloud EXCEPT letter X
Say letters as quickly as possible
Egas Moniz (1874-1955), a Portuguese physician, introduced prefrontal leucotomy for the relief of psychiatric disorders
Based on observations of temperament change (“calming” effect) in chimapanzees following frontal lobe lesions
First performed in 1935
For severe psychosis (eg, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder) for which there was no other treatment
Stopped following the introduction of antipsychotic medications in 1950’s
Moniz’s lobotomy was down in a surgical theatre, with the needle going down through the top of the head and into three areas, where it was twisted
Walter Freeman, USA, transorbital lobotomy from 1946 psych-surgery ‘by the bedside’ was done through going past the eyelid into the brain and moving the needle side to side. >18,000 patients in USA by 1951
Degeneration (loss of neurons) in the Frontal and Temporal lobes
2nd most common dementia (after Alzheimer’s disease)
Early symptoms difficult to distinguish from Alzheimer’s disease
Symptoms: (all important functions of the Frontal Lobes)
Disinhibition: Increasingly inappropriate actions. eg. impulsive behaviour, overeating, overly-sexual behaviour, lack of social “tact”, lack of care for appearance and personal hygeine
Apathy: Lack of motivation, emotionally distant, withdrawn (may appear like depression)
Loss of Empathy: Unaware of the emotions of others, lacking social skills, may become socially withdrawn
Deficits in Executive Functions (Neuropsychological Testing) Planning, reasoning, organisation of complex tasks or sequences
(Speech and Language, Motor deficits)
Frontal lobes: largest lobes, highly evolved in humans.
Crucial for executive functions:
Reasoning, planning, problem-solving.
Inhibitory control (suppressing inappropriate actions/urges).
Everyday control depends on top-down control (conscious goals) over bottom-up urges (unconscious drives).
Example: resisting cake on a diet (top-down inhibits bottom-up desire).
ADHD: impulsivity, distractibility.
OCD: compulsions driven by intrusive obsessions.
Addiction: compulsive behavior, impaired self-control.
Severe frontal lobe damage → loss of behavioral control: impulsive, inappropriate, profane behavior.
Early “treatment” for psychosis, especially schizophrenia (before antipsychotics).
Pioneered by Egas Moniz (Nobel Prize 1949).
Walter Freeman (USA): 18,000 patients by 1951.
Result: loss of frontal lobe-mediated behavioral control.
2nd most common dementia (after Alzheimer’s).
Progressive neuron loss in frontal and temporal lobes.
Behavioral variant:
Inappropriate, impulsive actions (overeating, inappropriate sexual behavior).
Loss of social tact and self-care.
Disinhibition in everyday life.
Neuropsychological testing: deficits in planning, reasoning, executive function.
Non-fluent/agrammatic variant (like Broca’s aphasia): effortful, halting speech.
Logopenic variant: word-finding pauses, effortful but good understanding.
Semantic dementia: fluent speech but empty content, loss of word knowledge.
Example:
Non-fluent: knows what a glass is but struggles to say “glass.”
Semantic dementia: can say “glass” but doesn’t understand its function.
Assess changes: behavior, language, cognition.
Executive functions: starting/stopping behavior, regulation, goal-setting.
Example Test:
Hayling Sentence Completion Test:
Fill in missing word → measures initiation.
Fill in with nonsense word → measures inhibition.
Patients with behavioral FTD: can’t inhibit automatic responses (e.g., keep saying “ship” instead of nonsense word).
Damage to frontal regions → impulsive decisions (e.g., impulse buying).
Examples:
Patient bought 200 pounds of fish.
Another patient impulsively bought 3 kitchens.
Key Insight:
Frontal lobes regulate behavior by balancing starting and stopping of actions → damage or disease disrupts this balance.
Learning = modification of behavior through experience.
No new behaviors emerge; existing behaviors change in strength.
Simple nervous system (~20,000 neurons).
Studied by Eric Kandel → foundational for neurobiology of learning.
Key anatomy:
Gill: breathing.
Siphon: expelling waste.
Gill withdrawal reflex: automatic retraction when disturbed.
Repeated stimulus exposure → decrease in response strength.
Procedure:
Firm water jet to siphon → measure gill withdrawal.
Repeat every ~90 seconds.
Gill withdrawal reflex weakens over time.
Adaptive: ignoring non-threatening, persistent stimuli conserves energy.
Exposure to strong aversive stimulus → increase in response strength.
Procedure:
Baseline: water jet to siphon → measure response.
Apply weak electric shock to tail.
Apply water jet again → stronger gill withdrawal than initially.
Adaptive: heightens vigilance after exposure to potential threat (“better safe than sorry”).
Associative learning: forming links between neutral stimuli and natural responses.
Pavlov’s dogs:
US (food) → UR (salivation).
CS (metronome) + US → eventually, CS alone elicits CR (salivation).
Key elements:
Unconditioned Stimulus (US)
Unconditioned Response (UR)
Conditioned Stimulus (CS)
Conditioned Response (CR)
Higher-order conditioning: CS1 (metronome) can condition CS2 (tennis ball) → weaker CR as removed further from US.
Learning via consequences of behavior (Law of Effect).
Thorndike’s puzzle boxes: cats learn which actions release latch faster.
BF Skinner’s operant chamber (Skinner box): allows controlled study of behavior → reinforcement/punishment.
Reinforcement: increases behavior frequency.
Punishment: decreases behavior frequency.
Positive: adding a stimulus.
Negative: removing a stimulus.
Type | Add/Remove | Outcome |
---|---|---|
Positive Reinforcement | Add pleasant | e.g., give food. |
Negative Reinforcement | Remove unpleasant | e.g., stop jackhammer noise. |
Positive Punishment | Add unpleasant | e.g., shock when light is off. |
Negative Punishment | Remove pleasant | e.g., remove sweet food access. |
1:1 ratio: every behavior is reinforced.
Fast learning, but not robust to extinction.
More robust learning, slower extinction.
Schedule Type | Based On | Fixed vs. Variable | Examples & Behavior Patterns |
---|---|---|---|
Interval | Time | Fixed: predictable timingVariable: unpredictable timing | Fixed interval: weekly pay (scalloped ramping up).Variable interval: pop quizzes (steady, moderate rate). |
Ratio | Responses | Fixed: set numberVariable: changing number | Fixed ratio: loyalty cards (stop-start pattern).Variable ratio: slot machines (high, constant responding). |
Variable ratio: highest, constant rate of responding.
Fixed ratio: stop-start responding after reinforcement.
Variable interval: steady, moderate responding.
Fixed interval: scalloped pattern – ramp up as interval ends.
Classical conditioning: links neutral cues with automatic responses.
Operant conditioning: links behavior with consequences → reinforcement/punishment.
Schedules of reinforcement: determine how consistently behavior is rewarded → affects response patterns and learning robustness.
Classical Conditioning Overview
Principles of Conditioning
Acquisition and Time-Sensitivity
Extinction (not forgetting!)
Spontaneous Recovery
Generalization & Discrimination
Classical Conditioning: Applications
Advertising
Fears, Phobias, and Little Albert
Behavioral Treatments for Phobias
Disgust and Taste Aversion
A conditioned response (CR) does not appear instantly.
Rather, it is gradually acquired by repeatedly pairing the UCS and CS.
Early learning is characterized by more rapid trial-by-trial changes in the CR.
Later on in learning, there is progressively less change in the CR, as it approaches an asymptote.
Both the rate and the asymptotic strength of the conditioned response depend on the relative timing of the CS and the UCS.
Learning is most efficient when the CS is presented shortly before the UCS.
Backward conditioning, presenting the UCS before the CS, is often ineffective.
Potential evolutionary significance: a Cause must occur before an Effect
A conditioned response will reduce in strength and eventually disappear if the CS is repeatedly presented without the UCS.
Extinction is a common way of eliminating a CR. Another way is to try and pair the CS with a new CR.
Important to note that extinction is not the same as forgetting a CS-CR pairing
If extinction produces forgetting—it should not be possible for a CR to reappear unless the CS-CR association has been retrained.
The previously trained CS-CR association remained intact
Potentially being suppressed by other more active associations
Presentation of the original learning context can trigger spontaneous reappearance of the CR
Stimuli that are similar to the CS will tend to elicit the same CR as the CS itself. When this happens, the CR has generalized to the novel stimulus.
If the stimulus is dissimilar from the CS, it will not elicit the CR. The organism is able to discriminate the CS from the new stimulus.
The Little Albert experiment, conducted by John B.
Watson and Rosalie Rayner in 1920, involved conditioning a young child named Albert to fear a white rat by pairing it with a loud, frightening noise. This demonstrated how emotional responses like fear can be classically conditioned in humans
Disgust towards certain stimuli can be viewed as a form of Classical Conditioning
Strong “one-trial” learning after food poisoning experiences
Biological rationale for these kinds of rapid learning
Learning via Reward and Punishment
Relative effectiveness of reward and punishment
Unintended consequences of punishment
Principles of Operant Conditioning
Relation to Classical Conditioning
Continuous vs. Partial Reinforcement: Humphrey’s Paradox
Applications of Operant Conditioning
Shaping and Complex Behavior
Revisiting Fears and Phobias
Superstition and Irrational Behavior
Reinforcement —outcome that increases the strength/frequency/probability of a behavior
Punishment — outcome that decreases the strength/frequency/probability of a behavior
What makes an outcome Reinforcing or Punishing depends on what has been added or removed from the environment
Reinforcement tends to train a target behavior more effectively than Punishment
The target behavior (sitting) elicits a good outcome (dog treat)
A non-target behavior (not sitting) elicits a bad outcome (shock)
Reinforcement is more focused and informative
Punishment can lead to unexpected outcomes
If a Punishment outcome can be anticipated by the learner, they may learn ways to avoid it
Potentially undesirable behaviours may be
Negatively Reinforced by removing a bad (future) outcome
Eliminating a previously reinforced behavior by no longer delivering reinforcement
Re-emergence of a previously reinforced behavior despite suspension of reinforcement
Whether a reinforced behaviour is emitted in response to similar, but not identical stimuli
Acquisition – Incrementally associating a behavior with an outcome
Timing rules for Classical Conditioning still apply – the closer in time a Behavior is followed by Reinforcement, the more rapidly learning will proceed
With Operant Conditioning, we need to consider how consistently reinforcement is being delivered to the learner
Continuous Reinforcement – every instance of the behavior is reinforced
Partial Reinforcement – only some instances of the behavior are reinforced
Humphrey's Paradox, proposed by Nicholas Humphrey in 1976, questions why self-regarding emotions like fear and pain exist if they do not directly aid survival.
It suggests that such feelings seem maladaptive since an organism could theoretically respond to danger without experiencing these emotions.
However, the paradox underscores how subjective experiences like fear might actually promote learning and caution in potentially dangerous situations, highlighting their adaptive value.
To train a target behavior, begin by reinforcing behaviors that vaguely approximate the target behavior. Then, restrict reinforcement to behaviors that are increasingly similar to the target behavior.
Simple behaviors can be chained together by reinforcement to form more complex behaviors
Fears that are acquired through Classical Conditioning may not passively resist extinction or forgetting…
Anxiety caused by the Conditioned Stimulus can be relieved by actively avoiding the stimulus.
Reduction in anxiety provoked by avoidance behavior Negatively Reinforces the fear.
Pigeon develops a “superstition” about the behaviors that seemingly provoked a food reward
Operant Conditioning can maintain superstitious behaviors (or rituals) via Positive and Negative Reinforcement
Memory: Multi-Store Model and Processes
Memory, or the retention of information over time, is often described using a multi-store model consisting of sensory memory, short-term memory (STM), and long-term memory (LTM). Encoding, storage, and retrieval are central to these processes.
Sensory Memory is fleeting and modality-specific. Iconic memory (visual) lasts less than a second and has a high capacity, while echoic memory (auditory) lasts 5–10 seconds but has lower capacity.
Short-Term Memory is not modality-specific. It holds about 7 ± 2 items (Miller’s “magical number”), lasting 20–30 seconds without rehearsal. Rehearsal refreshes STM contents and aids in transferring information to LTM.
Long-Term Memory is not capacity-limited and can last indefinitely. Forgetting here is more due to retrieval failures than storage losses.
Forgetting from STM has been explained by two major theories:
Decay: Information fades with time.
Interference: New information disrupts retrieval of older items. Research (e.g., Peterson & Peterson) found that both longer delays and competing tasks (e.g., backward counting) worsen recall, supporting interference as a major factor.
Free Recall and Serial Position Effects
Free recall experiments, where participants recall lists of words in any order, reveal a serial position curve:
Primacy effect: Early items are remembered better due to more rehearsal and transfer to LTM.
Recency effect: Last items are remembered well because they remain in STM.
Middle items: Recall is lowest because of minimal rehearsal and interference.
Classical Conditioning
Pioneered by Pavlov, this form of learning involves associating a neutral stimulus (e.g., a metronome) with an unconditioned stimulus (e.g., food) until the neutral stimulus alone elicits the conditioned response (e.g., salivation). This can extend to higher-order conditioning, where new neutral stimuli (e.g., a tennis ball) become conditioned by association with already conditioned stimuli.
Operant Conditioning
Building on Thorndike’s law of effect, B.F. Skinner formalized operant conditioning: learning driven by consequences.
Reinforcement (increases behavior) and punishment (decreases behavior) can be positive (adding a stimulus) or negative (removing a stimulus).
Reinforcement schedules:
Continuous reinforcement: Every response is reinforced.
Partial reinforcement: Only some responses are reinforced.
Fixed ratio: e.g., every 5th behavior.
Variable ratio: reinforcement after an unpredictable number of behaviors (e.g., gambling).
Fixed interval: reinforcement after a predictable time interval.
Variable interval: reinforcement after an unpredictable time interval (e.g., pop quizzes).
Variable schedules tend to produce steadier responding than fixed schedules.
Effective Retrieval and Memory Cues
Retrieval cues are any features of the environment that help trigger memories. They work by forming associations with the target information, increasing the chance of recall if direct retrieval fails.
The stronger the cue–target association, the better the retrieval performance.
Contextual Cues and Encoding Specificity
The principle of encoding specificity (Tulving) states that memory is improved when the retrieval context matches the encoding context.
Godden & Baddeley (1975) demonstrated this with divers recalling word lists better when tested in the same environment (land or underwater) as where they learned them.
Depth of Processing and Retrieval
The depth of processing framework (Craik & Lockhart) states that deeper, more meaningful encoding improves memory.
Shallow processing (e.g., noticing capital letters) yields poor recall.
Intermediate processing (e.g., considering rhymes) yields better recall.
Deep processing (e.g., thinking about uses of an object) produces the best recall.
Deeper processing creates more conceptual associations, which provide more retrieval cues.
Everyday Cues and Retrieval
Examples include driving: cues in the environment (e.g., a fork in the road) automatically trigger retrieval of appropriate behaviors (e.g., turning on the indicator).
Interference: When Cues Misfire
Sometimes, strong associations cause errors rather than aiding retrieval:
Proactive interference: Old memories disrupt new learning (e.g., using the indicator as in your old car when you get a new car with switched controls).
Retroactive interference: New learning disrupts old memories (e.g., struggling to recall a word in your native language after intense immersion in a second language).
Summary
Retrieval cues are essential for memory: they create multiple pathways to access stored information.
Deeper encoding and matched contexts strengthen cues.
However, strong but mismatched cues can interfere with retrieval, causing proactive or retroactive interference.
Theoretical Implications of the Multi-Store Model
Distinguishing Different Memory Stores
Measuring Sensory Memory
Measuring Short-Term Memory
Measuring Long-Term Memory
Information is successively transferred to different memory stores
Increasingly durable forms of memory
Several points where information can be lost
Transfer from sensory memory to short-term memory
Transfer from short-term memory to long-term memory
Fleeting awareness of the presence of lots of information…
…but it is difficult to report or describe all the details
Perhaps we have access to a very large amount of sensory information, but a very short window of opportunity to encode it into a more durable form
Sperling (1960) contrasted performance on Full Report and Partial Report versions of the memory task you just completed
Full Report performance was around 5 letters (50% of array)
Partial Report performance was virtually perfect (100% of cued array)
A large amount of information is stored in visual sensory memory (12+ items), but only for a short amount of time—otherwise Full Report would be as good as Partial Report!
Commonly measured using a span task
Study a list of letters/digits/words, presented at a rate of around 1 per second
Recall as many digits as you can in the order they were presented
List length when recall errors occur reflects storage capacity
Approximately 7 ± 2 items (Miller, 1956)
Duration of around 20-30 seconds or so
The effective shelf-life of information in Short-Term Memory can be increased through rehearsal
Memory span appears shorter when rehearsal is prevented
Perhaps the Magical Number 7 ± 2 is overly optimistic!
Cowan (2001) has argued that the capacity limit is actually closer to 4 ± 1
How does forgetting from short-term memory occur?
Temporal Decay – memories fade with the passage of time
Interference – memories become harder to distinguish from one another as they become more numerous
Capacity of Long-Term Memory is massive (perhaps unlimited?)
How many words do you know?
How many people do you know by name?
How many songs can you remember the lyrics to
How many movies, TV shows, etc. can you remember details of?
What about personal life events?
How many skills, actions, and movements can you perform?
Tested people’s memory for their High School graduation cohort using their own yearbooks as stimuli
Interval between graduation and testing ranged from 2 weeks to 57 years
Free Recall – Recall as many full names as you can in 8 minutes
Recognition of Names – Do you recognize this name? (yes/no)
Recognition of Portraits – Do you recognize this person? (yes/no)
Matching Names to Portraits (two versions)
Cued Recall – Who is this?
Variation in difficulty across tests. Some require more specific information than others (e.g., Matching requires memory of names-to-faces, whereas Recognition of Names/Portraits do not)
Matching performance and recognition of partial information is very good!
Free Recall and Cued Recall fare worse, but are still remarkably good!
Only major drop-off occurs after a retention interval of nearly 50 years
Cue-based retrieval
Mnemonic strategies: From Simple Cues to Complex Chunking
Can memory be trained?
Beyond Pure Retrieval: Memory as a Reconstructive Process
Schemas
Misinformation
False Memory
Method of Loci
Associate memoranda with locations/landmarks along a familiar route
Pegword Technique
“One is a bun, two is a shoe, three is a tree, etc.”
Pairing vivid imagery with memoranda
Keyword Method
Pair a word that sounds similar to the to-be-remembered target
Elements within a group become associated with each other
They can form cohesive (if not meaningful) units
Retrieval of one part triggers retrieval of the whole unit
Daily training: 1-Up, 2-Down
Start with a 5-item list
Tested one participant, SF, for 260+ hours over 2 years
List length can only increase with consistent recall
End of Day 1
Best recall: 7 items
Pure Rehearsal
End of Day 4
Best recall: 9 items
Group by 3s
End of Day 264
Best recall: 82 items
Complex Chunking
SF was an avid longdistance runner—with deep knowledge of times
Sequences of numbers remembered in terms of running times in different races (e.g., half-mile, marathon, etc.)
SF was not an exception to the rule. Another runner, DD, was recruited and trained to use SF’s method. DD’s span was 68 items after 268 hours
We mostly think of memory as a “reactive” process – we selectively retrieve information that’s been held in storage.
But there is good reason to believe that memory is also an “active” process that involves an element of reconstruction.
Sometimes what comes out never actually went in!
Schemas
Templates or “scripts” for familiar situations
If the details of something are forgotten (or not encoded), the “default” offered by the script can be inserted into one’s memory for what happened
Relatives of participants provided experimenter with information on 3 events that happened when the participant was around 5 years old
A fourth story, introduced by the experimenter, was about the participant getting lost at the shops for a long time
Around 25% of participants reported “remembering” being lost—some even provided additional details about the event
Multiple factors erode confidence in eyewitness memory.
Suggestibility
Schemas
Reconstructive nature of memory demands caution on the part of law enforcement agencies and jurors.
Memory for specific facts—information you are aware of recalling
Procedural or Implicit Memory
Memory for doing things—may not be tied to conscious awareness
Episodic Memory
Memory for specific events or scenes from one’s life
Semantic Memory
Information about general knowledge that is housed in Long-Term Memory
Problem Solving: Key Approaches
Overview:
Problem solving involves moving from an initial state to a goal state, often through multiple interacting steps and potential pathways. Solutions can be reached either incrementally or suddenly (insight).
Two Main Approaches:
Incremental Problem Solving:
Involves a gradual, step-by-step progression.
Example: Thorndike’s Cats – They learned to escape puzzle boxes through repeated, incremental improvements, without sudden insight.
Insight Problem Solving:
Involves sudden, seemingly effortless leaps to the solution (“aha!” moments).
Example: Archimedes’ Bath – Eureka moment realizing volume can be measured by water displacement.
Kohler’s Chimps – Sudden stacking of crates to reach bananas.
Computational Perspective:
Popularized by Newell & Simon (1970s), focusing on incremental approaches.
Problem Solving Algorithms:
Fixed, step-by-step procedures guaranteeing a solution.
Often slow and computationally demanding.
Example: Following step-by-step Lego instructions.
Problem Solving Heuristics:
Flexible, “rules of thumb” that are faster but may not always work.
Example: Building Lego walls and roof based on experience.
Problem Space and Navigation:
Problem solving as navigating from an initial state (pile of Lego bricks) to a goal state (completed house).
Actions (algorithmic or heuristic) incrementally change the problem state.
Two Noteworthy Heuristics (Newell & Simon):
Means-Ends Analysis:
Breaks down the problem into smaller, manageable sub-goals.
Example: Build four walls, then a roof.
Hill-Climbing:
No explicit solution known in advance.
Progress by always choosing the next step that most improves the current state.
Limitation: Can get stuck at a “local maximum” that is not the true solution (like stopping at a high point on a foggy hill that isn’t the summit).
Conclusion:
Insight and incremental approaches are both central to problem solving.
Incremental approaches include reliable algorithms and faster, flexible heuristics.
Heuristics are versatile but can lead to suboptimal or incorrect solutions.
Effective problem solving involves weighing these approaches based on the problem’s demands and available resources.
Classical view of concepts
Necessary & sufficient features
Challenges to the classical view
Conceptual structure: typicality & prototype theory, ad hoc categories, exceptions & exemplar theory
Key topics: similarity, hierarchical structure, and concept use
Mental representations organizing knowledge about specific things (real or imaginary)
Help identify what things are and their properties
Concepts defined by necessary and sufficient features
Something either is an example of the concept or not
Positive examples possess necessary features
Examples: all birds have feathers, hatch, and fly
No features shared by all examples (Wittgenstein, 1953)
Example: “games” lack a single defining feature
People may not recognize category members despite knowing defining features (Hampton, 1979)
E.g., inconsistent categorization of vegetables
Typicality ratings (Rosch, 1975) show categories are graded, not all-or-none
Examples share many (but not all) features; overlapping features differ across examples
Typical category members share more features with other category members
Less typical members share fewer features with others
High family resemblance stimuli learned faster and rated as more typical (Rosch & Mervis, 1975)
Concepts can be constructed on-the-fly for goals (ad hoc categories; Barsalou, 1983)
Ad hoc categories show typicality and family resemblance
Prototypes viewed as an ideal, average, list of common features, or single most typical example
Typicality depends on proximity to the prototype and shared features
Posner & Keele (1968): more prototypical stimuli classified faster and remembered better
Similarity to multiple exemplars more influential than prototype similarity (Shepard’s exponential generalization law)
Handling category exceptions is difficult (e.g., bats resemble bird prototype more than mammal prototype)
Concepts as collections of memorized instances (category exemplars)
Membership determined by overall similarity to category exemplars
Typical stimuli highly similar to many other category exemplars
Central to both prototype and exemplar theories
Conceptualized as distance in psychological space
Triangle inequality (Tversky, 1977): may not hold if different contexts affect similarity judgments
Superordinate and subordinate categories (e.g., animals → dogs → beagles)
Features at higher levels are present at lower levels (supports inference)
Typicality influences whether features generalize from typical to atypical exemplars
Intermediate category level preferred (e.g., “tree” over “pine” or “plant”)
Informs and differentiates objects
Influenced by culture and expertise (Medin et al., 1997)
Experts (e.g., tree taxonomists) structure knowledge differently than laypeople
Concepts organize knowledge, enabling identification and inference
Classical definitional views replaced by prototypes and exemplars
Typicality and family resemblance essential
Key themes: similarity, hierarchical structure, interactions between structure and use
Heuristics and cognitive biases
Heuristics reveal how cognition operates
Subjective expected utility framework (Von Neumann & Morgenstern, 1944) is effortful
Example: cost-benefit trade-offs in housing decisions
Weighs pros and cons of marriage
Example of decision-making complexities
Simple rules of thumb (heuristics) simplify decision-making
May not produce optimal decisions but are fast and effective
Exploring what heuristics reveal about cognition
When little information is available, choose the recognized option
Effective when recognition correlates with the target attribute
Judgments based on similarity to a cognitive prototype
E.g., Linda as feminist bank teller; random sequence illusions
Leads to conjunction fallacy (bank teller vs. feminist bank teller)
Ignoring statistical base rates in favor of descriptive information (e.g., Jack at party)
Decisions based on how easily instances come to mind (e.g., homicide vs. suicide rates)
Order effects in math problems (anchoring and adjustment)
Labels (e.g., “friendly” or “rude”) bias impression formation
Decisions depend on how problems are framed (e.g., “200 saved” vs. “400 die”)
Departures from rational choice
Influence of memory and information priority
Heuristics offer timely, usually effective decisions
Perception: More Than Our Senses
We do not see with our eyes, hear with our ears, or smell with our noses. Our sense organs only transduce environmental energy into signals that our brain then processes. What we experience—sight, sound, smell, taste, and touch—are products of brain activity, not direct impressions of the world.
Sensory Adaptation & Aftereffects
Sensory systems adapt to prolonged exposure, reducing responsiveness to constant inputs.
Example: The rotating spiral illusion, or the “motion aftereffect.”
Visual adaptation enhances salience of new inputs.
Troxler fading: Inputs fade when fixated upon, causing aftereffects and illusions (like the “crimson chaser” illusion).
Adaptation occurs in all sensory modalities and ensures that new, novel stimuli stand out.
Multimodal Integration & the McGurk Effect
The McGurk Effect shows how audio and visual inputs combine to create a perceptual experience that differs from either alone.
Perception is not simply veridical—it is constructed by the brain from multimodal evidence.
Perception as Construction, Not Reflection
Perceptual filling-in: We “complete” missing parts of our vision (e.g., the physiological blind spot) using contextual cues.
Neon colour spreading demonstrates how our brain constructs percepts from available evidence.
This reveals the naïve realism fallacy—believing that we perceive the world exactly as it is.
Transduction in Hearing & Vision
Hearing: Soundwaves cause air molecules to compress and rarefy.
Frequency = pitch.
Amplitude = loudness.
Hair cells in the cochlea detect these vibrations and convert them into electrochemical signals.
Vision: Light is transduced by photoreceptors in the retina.
Photoreceptors (rods and cones) absorb photons and trigger electrical signals that travel to the brain via the optic nerve.
However, the human eye is limited by cellular layers that blur images and create a blind spot.
Colour Vision: Individual Differences & Species Variability
Humans: Three types of cones (trichromatic vision).
Short cones (blue), medium cones (green), long cones (red).
Some people lack one cone type (red-green colour blindness), others have a fourth cone (potentially superhuman colour vision).
Birds and goldfish see beyond human limits—ultraviolet and infrared.
Idiosyncratic variation: Even people with “normal” trichromatic vision differ in their cone ratios, possibly causing unique perceptions.
Colour Constancy: Accounting for Lighting
The brain adjusts colour perception to discount the effect of illumination—colour constancy.
Example: The viral dress photo (white and gold or blue and black) demonstrates how perception of colour can depend on assumptions about lighting.
Key Takeaway:
Perception is not a perfect record of reality.
Our brains construct perceptions based on sensory input, past experiences, and contextual information.
This is why your experience of “red” might not be exactly the same as mine, even though we share the same physical environment.
The world’s first Experimental Psychology lab was established by Wilhelm Wundt in Leipzig, Germany, in 1879.
Initially a physiologist, Wundt wanted to understand human perception…
He developed a method he called introspection
Wundt & Helmholtz knew that we see with our brains, and that we don’t see all that meets our eyes, because they were well aware of a host of visual phenomena
By applying the scientific method to study sensation/perception, Wundt created a setting where other Phenomena could be studied
This includes all the diverse phenomena studied in contemporary psychological science
“objects in space around us appear to possess the qualities of our sensations. They appear to have an odor or a taste, and so on. Yet these qualities of sensations belong only to our nervous system… Even when we know this, however, the illusion does not cease” Helmholtz 1878: The facts of perception
By applying the scientific method to study sensation/perception, Wundt and Helmholtz were contributing to the over throw of centuries of misconception…
The human mind had been thought to be separate from the human brain…
The human brain was thought to be material, and therefore measurable. The human mind, however, had been thought to be immaterial
The philosopher Rene Descartes had supported this concept, called dualism. He thought the brain communicated with the mind by transmitting signals out into the ether via the pineal gland
As sensation and perception had been thought to be products of the mind, they were considered irrevocably mysterious, and beyond measure!
The suggestion that we could measure perception was controversial
It was suggested that setting up an experimental psychology lab…
“would insult religion by putting the human soul in a pair of scales.” University of Cambridge Senate, 1877
Sensation: An ability to detect sensory input. As you will see next week, your central nervous system can detect many signals that you may never become aware of…
Perception: Your subjective experiences of sensory input.
e.g. the feeling of red, or the experience of roughness on your skin as your brush a rough surface, or the experience of a particular musical note
As you have begun to see in our online content, Wundt & von Helmholtz were aware of a number of situations where our brains/minds generate experiences that are additional to the retinal images that reach our eyes…
In a very real sense, your brain makes things up
Our brains have no direct access to information concerning the external visual environment!
To ‘see’ the visual system must detect electromagnetic radiation, and convert it into neural events
Ultimately, our brains infer what these neural events are signalling regarding our surrounds
The conversion of electromagnetic radiation into neural events. Involves light sensitive chemicals - visual pigments
Visual pigments are contained in outer segments of photoreceptors that form the retina
Pigments absorb photons of light, beginning a process that changes photoreceptor membrane conductance, causing depolarisation and action potentials
The conversion of electromagnetic radiation into neural events. Involves light sensitive chemicals - visual pigments
Visual pigments are contained in outer segments of photoreceptors that form the retina
Pigments absorb photons of light, beginning a process that changes photoreceptor membrane conductance, causing depolarisation and action potentials
Action potentials propagate to retinal Ganglion cells, via Horizontal, Bipolar & Amacrine cells, forward through retina
Output from retinal ganglion cells combine to form the optic nerve – that carries signals out of your retinae to your brain
To reach your brain, the optic nerve must pass through your retinae
Each photoreceptor maximally absorbs a specific wavelength of light, with absorbance tapering off as wavelengths increasingly change from optimal
The range of wavelengths that can be absorbed form the visible spectrum – the ‘light’ that you can see
The point at which the optic nerve leaves your eye is called the optic disc.
At the topic disc there can be no photoreceptors – so you are blind to images that project to that position on your retinae
As you are BLIND to images that project to your optic disc, it is often called the blindspot!
There are several reasons people don’t typically notice their blindspots, but the most interesting is that the human visual system seems to assume that the same things that surround the blindspot are also within the blindspot – a process called perceptual filling in
Another situation where your brain causes you to see things that are not present on the retinae are Coloured Aftereffects (which were well known to Helmholtz and Wundt)
The fact that you can see colours, when none are physically present, shows that you are seeing activity in your brain, rather than retinal images, or a veridical impression of the external world!
Coloured Aftereffects helped reveal what wavelengths of light cones are maximally responsive to many decades before we had the requisite technology to study photoreceptors
Opponent Process theory – After protracted viewing of certain colours, you can see oppositely coloured afterimages. This allowed colour vision scientists to infer that the human visual system contains competitive mechanisms tuned to opposite colours…
Sensory adaptation does not just impact low-level processes in your retinae. It can impact perception of complex visual features – like the gender of human faces
The gender face aftereffect shows that even our experiences of well known complex forms are subject to change – shaped by visual adaptation.
Perception is a dynamic construction of your brain / mind
Aftereffects occur, in part, because the firing rates of neurons that are responsive to an input ‘adapt’ over time, which means they become less responsive
In an opponent code, neurons are maximally responsive to opposite features – such as to red and to green
Note that cells are maximally responsive to one type of input, and are less responsive to a wide range of other inputs
Prior to adaptation, cells that are maximally responsive to opposite colours would be equally responsive to an intermediate colour ‘grey’
2-channel opponent code If the visual system is adapted to green, cells that are maximally responsive to green become less responsive…
Then if you are exposed to grey, neurons that are maximally responsive to the opposite colour red will become relatively more responsive
Unbalanced responses to inputs due to adaptation is what causes perceptual aftereffects…
Your experiences of physical inputs depends on the responses of millions of neurons, and these change depending on what they have been adapted to
Visual adaptation is one way to modulate the responses of cells to inputs. Attention is another.
The basketball demo works because feature based attention causes your visual system to be less responsive to unattended colours (in this case, to black)
You can also have spatial and time based attention
Perception is not simply determined by the physical lights that are refracted from a given surface!
Colour Constancy: In different conditions the colour of the same wavelength of light can look very different
The human brain facilitates a sense of colour constancy, by estimating what the prevailing light source is, and subtracting its influence from your impression of object colours
To understand this, you have to appreciate that a range of wavelengths are reflected from any given surface, and this range is shifted by the light source
You can use the brains’ tendency to estimate the prevailing light source to reverse apparent colour
You can use the brains’ tendency to estimate the prevailing light source to reverse apparent colour and contrast polarity (brightness)
Some people’s visual systems infer that ‘the dress’ is lit by a warm light source (the sun). Their brains subtract a ‘yellow’ illuminant, creating a veridical impression of the dress
Others visual systems infer a cool illuminant (an indoor light source), creating an impression of a tan/gold and white dress
To understand why, you need to know a little bit about normal human daytime vision
Usually, humans use 3 types of ‘photoreceptor’ which catch photons carried by different wavelengths of light, and turns these into signals that are sent to your brain
A very small number of people (girls) have an extra class of day time photoreceptor
These girls may have ‘super’ human colour vision – able to distinguish many more colours than others!
We all have different concentrations of daytime photoreceptors…
Some people may experience green colours as brighter than red, and others the reverse. But we all learn colours by association, so normally we could never tell if this were true
Signals travel from the eye via the optic nerve to the optic chiasm.
At the chiasm, signals cross: right visual field signals go to left brain hemisphere; left visual field signals go to right hemisphere.
This crossing is independent of which eye the signals come from.
Damage before optic chiasm affects one eye; damage after affects one visual field side, indicating brain damage.
Signals continue to lateral geniculate nuclei (LGNs) and then to primary visual cortex (V1) in occipital lobe.
V1 located in occipital cortex, one in each hemisphere.
Each V1 maps the opposite side of visual field retinotopically (adjacent neurons correspond to adjacent retina regions).
Foveal (central) vision mapped at posterior V1; peripheral vision maps more anteriorly.
Vertical retinotopic mapping: lower visual field maps to upper V1 and vice versa.
Damage to V1 causes cortical scotomas—blind spots corresponding to damaged visual field areas.
Not all V1 activity leads to conscious visual perception.
Flicker fusion threshold (~30Hz for color) limits conscious detection of rapidly changing stimuli, but V1 cells respond above this threshold.
Some V1 responses are subliminal; brain processes visual information not consciously perceived.
Occurs after V1 damage causing cortical blindness.
Patients report blindness in affected visual field regions but retain unconscious visual sensitivity.
Demonstrated with forced-choice tasks where patients guess stimulus features above chance despite denying seeing them.
Examples include Helen (monkey with bilateral V1 removal) and human patient PN navigating obstacles without conscious sight.
V1 necessary for conscious vision but not all visual processing.
Visual sensitivity can persist without conscious awareness.
Shows subjective reports of vision can be unreliable.
Experiments include signal-present and signal-absent (catch) trials.
Participant responses categorized as hits, misses, false alarms, and correct rejections.
Sensitivity measured by d-prime: ratio of hit rate to false alarm rate; d-prime = 0 means no sensitivity.
Sensitivity >0 means hit rate > false alarm rate.
Used to objectively measure sensitivity despite subjective uncertainty or blindness (e.g., blindsight).
Practical applications include fingerprint matching and airport security screening.
Experts show bias: prefer misses (false negatives) over false alarms (false positives) to avoid wrongful decisions.
Training in security introduces fake signals to reduce complacency and maintain detection sensitivity.
Visual neurons have receptive fields (retinal regions eliciting response).
Response selectivity: neurons respond to specific stimulus features (e.g., orientation, motion).
Example: a V1 cell responds strongly to vertical bars regardless of color, motion, or length, but not to slanted/horizontal bars.
Early research found retinal and LGN neurons respond to spots of light, but cortical neurons do not.
Accidental discovery by Hubel and Wiesel: cortical neurons respond to oriented edges/bars, not spots.
Nobel Prize awarded for discovering orientation selectivity in V1 neurons.
Simple features (bars, orientation) encoded in V1.
More complex features (motion patterns, object categories, faces) processed in higher visual cortical areas downstream of V1.
Processing hierarchy builds complexity from basic retinal inputs to advanced object recognition.