Motor Development: Fundamentals, Theoretical Perspectives, and Motion & Stability
Chapter 1: Fundamental Concepts
Learning objectives (Page 2)
- Define motor development
- Distinguish developmental issues from other concerns
- Describe some of the basic tools used by researchers in motor development
- Explain why development occurs over a life span
- Illustrate a model that guides the discussion of motor development
Characteristics of Motor Development (Page 3)
- Change in movement behavior
- Continuity
- Age-related progression
- Sequential order
- Depends on underlying processes
Related Areas of Study (Page 4)
- Motor learning: relatively permanent gains in motor skill capability associated with practice or experience
- Motor control: the neural, physical, and behavioral aspects of movement (Schmidt & Lee, 2014)
Related Terms (Page 5)
- Physical growth: quantitative increase in size or body mass;
\text{growth} = \text{size or mass increase} - Physical maturation: qualitative advance in biological makeup; cell, organ, or system advancement in biochemical composition (Teeple, 1978)
- Aging: process occurring with passage of time, leading to loss of adaptability or full function and eventually to death (Spirduso, Francis, & MacRae, 2005)
- Physical growth: quantitative increase in size or body mass;
Defining Motor Development (Page 6)
- Identify similarities and differences between motor development and the following phenomena:
- Motor learning
- Motor control
- Physical growth and maturation
Newell’s Model of Constraints (Figure 1.1) (Page 7)
- Constraints categories: Structural (Individual), Functional (Individual), Task, Environmental
- Constraints interact to shape movement
Constraints (Page 8)
- Constraints discourage or limit certain movements
- Constraints encourage or permit other movements
- “Shape” movement — channel away from some movements while towards others
Individual Constraints (Page 9)
- Unique physical, mental characteristics – Internal
- Structural: related to body’s structure (e.g., height, muscle mass)
- Functional: related to behavioral function (e.g., attention, motivation)
Environmental Constraints (Page 10)
- Properties of the environment – External
- Global, not task-specific
- Physical: gravity, surfaces
- Sociocultural: gender roles, cultural norms
Task Constraints (Page 11)
- Specific task requirements or goals – External
- Not related to the individual; related specifically to tasks or skills
- Include goal of task, rules guiding task performance, equipment
Interaction of Constraints (Page 12)
- Must identify individual and environmental and task constraints
- Must examine interactions among constraints
Video Example 1.1 (Page 13)
- Prompt: What are the important individual, environmental, and task constraints in this video?
Constraints and Atypical Development (Page 14)
- Disabilities: differences in structural, functional individual constraints
- Must consider all interacting constraints
- May result in delayed, different motor development
Video Example 1.2 (Page 15)
- Prompt: What constraints change between these two clips?
- (Click images to view videos)
Research Designs in Motor Development (Page 16)
- Longitudinal: an individual or group is observed over time; may require lengthy observation
- Cross-sectional: individuals or groups of different ages are observed; change inferred, not directly observed
- Sequential or mixed longitudinal: mini-longitudinal studies with overlapping ages
Model of Sequential Research Design (Figure 1.4) (Page 17)
- Demonstrates how cohorts are observed over time with overlapping ages
- Combines cross-sectional and longitudinal approaches to study development
Research Designs: Rationale (Page 18)
- Why use a longitudinal design to study motor development?
- Why use a cross-sectional design to study motor development?
A Paradox in Development (Page 19)
- Universality: great similarity in development across individuals within a species
- Variability: individual differences exist
Summary and Synthesis (Page 20)
- 1) Motor development examines continuous, sequential, age-related change in motor behavior
- 2) Underlying change is shaped by individual, environmental, and task constraints
- 3) Researchers use longitudinal, cross-sectional, and sequential designs to investigate this change
Chapter 2: Theoretical Perspectives in Motor Development
Learning objectives (Page 22)
- Describe theories used to study motor development
- Illustrate how various theories explain changes in motor behavior
- Describe the history of the field of motor development
Figure 2.1: Motor Development: What Happens? (Page 23)
- Conceptual view of how motor development unfolds across life span
Theories of Motor Development (Page 24)
- Maturational perspective
- Information processing perspective
- Ecological perspective
Maturational Perspective (Page 25)
- Motor development driven by maturation of systems (Nature)
- Specifically, Central Nervous System (CNS) maturation
- Minimal influence of environment
- Characteristics: qualitative, discontinuous
History of the Maturational Perspective (Page 26)
- 1930s: Gesell, McGraw proposed invariable, genetically determined sequence of development (timing may vary)
- Co-twin control strategy used in research
Maturationists’ Interest in Process (Page 27)
- McGraw (1935): motor behavior changes linked to nervous system development
- Advancement in CNS triggers appearance of a new skill
Long-Lasting Beliefs from Maturation Theory (Page 28)
- Basic motor skills emerge automatically
- Little need for special training
- Mild deprivation does not arrest development
- Nervous system deemed most important
Descriptive Methodology in Motor Development (Page 29)
- Characteristic of maturational perspective (1940–1970s)
- Normative: quantitative scores describing average performance (e.g., Espanchade, Glassow, Rarick)
- Biomechanical: movement-pattern descriptions in fundamental skills (e.g., Glassow, Halverson, Wickstrom)
How Would a Maturational Theorist Explain the Following? (Page 30)
- Toddler learning to walk
- Child riding a bike
- Teenager having difficulty swimming
Information Processing Perspective (Page 31)
- Motor development driven by external processes (Nurture)
- Brain viewed as a computer: passive human responds to environmental stimuli
- Key concepts: Input, encoding, processing, feedback
- Young adults often studied first as a basis for comparing performance across ages
Perceptual-Motor Development (Page 32)
- Subfield within information processing
- 1960s: early work linked learning disabilities to delayed perceptual-motor development
How Would an Information Processing Theorist Explain the Following? (Page 33)
- Toddler learning to walk
- Child riding a bike
- Teenager having difficulty swimming
Ecological Perspective (Page 34)
- Development driven by the interrelationship of the individual, environment, and task
- Importance of multiple systems
- Neural system is one of many systems contributing to action
- Two branches: Dynamical systems; Perception–action
Dynamical Systems (Page 35)
- Theory advanced in early 1980s by Kugler, Kelso, Turvey, and others
- Body systems spontaneously self-organize; not driven solely by CNS
- Interactions among body systems, performer, environment, and task demands
Dynamical Systems (continued) (Page 36)
- Some systems may develop more slowly in the young or degrade faster in the old, influencing rate of development or change
- Development characterized by qualitative and discontinuous change
- Change occurs across the life span
System Diagram (Figure 2.3) (Page 37)
- Illustrates multiple interacting systems over time
Perception–Action (Page 38)
- Based on Gibson’s work (1960s–1970s)
- Affordance: function an environmental object provides to an individual
- Object meanings defined by intrinsic dimensions (body-scaled) rather than extrinsic, objective dimensions
Ecological Perspective (Page 39)
- Both branches reject CNS as sole executive controller
- Control is distributed throughout the body, at global and local levels
- Enables new experiments and new ways of thinking about old questions
How Would an Ecological Perspective Theorist Explain the Following? (Page 40)
- Toddler learning to walk
- Child riding a bike
- Teenager having difficulty swimming
Summary and Synthesis (Page 41)
- 1) Multiple theoretical perspectives have emerged over time
- 2) Each perspective has benefits; there is value in viewing motor development from different angles
- 3) The maturational perspective has historically influenced understanding, but the ecological perspective is more widely used today
Chapter 3: Principles of Motion and Stability
Lesson objectives (Page 43)
- Outline the principles of motion and stability that lead to proficient motor performance
- Discuss relationships between these principles and motor behaviors across skill levels
- Explain how skilled performers take advantage of principles
Developmental Changes Are Predictable (Page 44)
- Based on optimizing biomechanical principles of motion and stability over time
- Observed across a variety of motor skills
- Often produce more force, velocity, or accuracy
Motion and Stability (Page 45)
- Developmental changes in movement follow biomechanical principles
- Motion and stability are two principles within biomechanics; the "physics" of movement
Video 3.1: What Changes From A to B? (Page 46)
- Visual prompt to compare two states of movement
Newton’s First Law (Page 47)
- An object at rest stays at rest; an object in motion stays in motion until acted upon by a force
- Inertia: resistance to motion related to mass
- Momentum: product of mass and velocity
- Equations:
{p = m\,v}
${}$
Newton’s First Law, Simplified (Page 48)
- We must exert force to move objects and to move ourselves
- Higher inertia means it is harder to move; more force is required
Newton’s First Law: Child Learns to Swing a Bat (Page 49)
- What must the child learn about inertia?
- What must the child learn about momentum? (p = m v)
To Move an Object Farther or Faster (Page 50)
- Increase force delivered to object
- Increase distance over which force is applied
Video 3.2: Adding Distance to Improve a Kick (Page 51)
- Increase step length (linear distance)
- Increase range of motion (rotational distance)
Newton’s Second Law (Page 52)
- Object’s force relates to mass and acceleration: F = m a
- Object’s acceleration relates to force and inversely to mass: a = \frac{F}{m}
- (continued)
Newton’s Second Law (continued) (Page 53)
- People can throw only as hard as they can throw
- Given this peak force, how could you increase acceleration when throwing a ball?
Newton’s Third Law (Page 54)
- To every action, there is an equal and opposite reaction
- When you push on something, it pushes back on you
Using Newton's Third Law (Page 55)
- Oppositional movements and directional force
Video 3.3: Force Generation Aided by Oppositional Movements (Page 56)
- Visual demonstration of opposing movements aiding force
Video 3.4: Exert Force in Primary Movement Plane (Page 57)
- Use force in the plane of motion in which you want to move yourself or an object
- Avoid rotational movements that reduce force in the desired plane
Increasing Velocity: Rotating Limbs and Projected Objects (Page 58)
- Increase rotational velocity (swing it faster)
- Increase relative length (fully extend at release or contact)
Video 3.5: Why Not Keep Limb Extended Throughout? (Page 59)
- The leg would have too much rotational inertia
Force and Time (Page 60)
- To move an object, increase force application for a given time
- Example: karate chop to bricks
- To stop an object, increase time over which a given force is applied
- Example: soft landing in gymnastics
Question (Page 61)
- What developmental skills involve learning to absorb force?
Stability and Balance (Page 62)
- Stability: ability to resist movement
- Balance: ability to maintain equilibrium
- Stability–mobility trade-off
Increasing Stability (Page 63)
- Increase the base of support
- Lower the center of gravity
Increasing Balance (Page 64)
- Increase stability
- Improve strength, coordination, and proprioception
Summary and Synthesis (Page 65)
- 1) Motion and stability are two biomechanical principles that constrain how individuals interact with the environment during task performance
- 2) Understanding and practicing motion and stability can lead to better control and performance of motor skills
- 3) Stability and balance are key mechanical principles in efficient and skilled movement