Novel environment hypothesis: The current environment differs significantly from the environment in which the behavior evolved, so species haven't had enough time to adapt.
Net benefit hypothesis: Sensory mechanisms may cause fitness losses for some individuals sometimes, but the average fitness gain from reacting to a sender outweighs these losses.
Thynnine wasps pollinate elbow orchids because the orchids send illegitimate signals.
Elbow orchids exploit male wasps' ability to respond to olfactory and visual cues of female wasps.
The orchid's lip petal emits odors similar to those released by calling females, attracting sexually motivated males.
Illegitimate senders can evolve if there's a legitimate communication system to exploit.
Orchids exploit a preexisting sensory bias in male wasps that evolved sensitivity to the appearance and odor of conspecific females.
The plant mimics the appearance and odor of female wasps.
Plants that feed rather than deceive pollinators evolved "come-and-get-it" signals after pollinators' sensory capacities evolved.
Male Photinus fireflies can be eaten when predators deceptively lure them to their deaths.
Calling tĂşngara frogs sometimes attract fringe-lipped bats instead of females.
Predators that take advantage of prey signals are illegitimate receivers, listening in on individuals that lose fitness.
Key difference between firefly and tĂşngara frog examples: [unclear from the context]
The risk of attack to male frogs is greater if the male call includes one or more chucks as well as the intro whine.
Males in small groups are more likely to drop the chucks from their calls compared to males in larger groups due to greater risk of predation.
Ground-nesting birds that encounter more mammalian predators tend to produce higher frequency calls that do not travel as far, better concealing the individual senders.
Clay eggs advertised by begging calls of the tree-nesting black-throated blue warbler were found and bitten significantly more often by eastern chipmunks than were the eggs associated with the calls of the ground-nesting ovenbird.
The two sexes of satin bowerbird look different and take on very dissimilar approaches to reproduction
In most animals, males do the courting and females do the choosing
Why do extravagant courtship behaviors and morphological traits (e.g., ornaments and armaments) persist over evolutionary time?
Sexual selection – “the advantage certain individuals have over others of the same sex and species in exclusive relation to reproduction” (Darwin 1871)
Reproductive skew – unequal partitioning of reproduction within a population or social group; the sex with higher skew also tends to have greater variance in reproductive success
In satin bowerbirds, there is high skew and reproductive variance among males, but much lower skew and reproductive variance among females, why?
Very few (if any) females use the sperm of more than one male to fertilize their eggs
Why is it more common for males to do the courting, and females to do the choosing?
Anisogamy – the occurrence of gametes that differ greatly in size, which generates many observable sex differences
In fruit flies, a male’s reproductive success increases with the number of mates; also referred to as the Bateman gradient
A female’s reproductive success is more dependent on partner quality (and ability to produce eggs and care for offspring) than on the number of male mates
Bateman’s principle – males tend to have higher reproductive variance than females as a result of sex differences in mating behavior
Slope of the gradient is indicative of the strength of sexual selection that a given sex experiences
Patricia Gowaty was unable to replicate Bateman’s experiments; she concluded that his method produced biased offspring number estimates of each sex by overestimating subjects with no mates and underestimated subjects with one or more mates
Gowaty’s findings do not eliminate the key insights previously outlined by Bateman:
Higher variance in the number of mates and in offspring production are both indicative of the sex experiencing stronger sexual selection
Bateman gradients are typically steeper in the sex experiencing stronger sexual selection
These insights apply equally well to both sexes; e.g., the same principles that act on male traits also act on female traits in many species
Spermatogenesis is not limitless and ejaculation can be costly
Males of many species need to recover after ejaculating and often have fewer sperm in subsequent ejaculations
In Soay sheep, predicted copulation rates (based on a male’s horn and hindleg length) were negatively related to the number of sperm per ejaculate
Sex differences do not end with the investment in gametes and reproductive behavior; they also carry over to investment in offspring care
Putting resources into large gametes and helping offspring become adults increases the probability that offspring will live long enough to reproduce and pass on parental genes to the next generation
What a parent supplies to one offspring cannot be used to make additional offspring down the road; the parent must make a potential trade-off in current versus future reproduction
Parental investment – the weighing of time/energy expenditures and risks taken by a parent to help existing offspring at the expense of reducing future reproductive opportunities
Parental investment takes many forms
In most species, females are more likely to derive a net benefit from taking care of offspring because it is extremely likely to carry her genes
A male’s paternity is often less certain, given that females of many species are inseminated by more than one male; males have less incentive to be parental if they lose fertilization opportunities in the process
Although males and females of diploid species may have the same average reproductive success, they will differ in reproductive variance due to divergence in reproductive skew
There are generally fewer sexually active females at any given time, creating a male-biased operational sex ratio
Females that have already mated usually have less to gain by subsequent copulations (or because it is more energetically expensive to replenish eggs than sperm)
Sex differences in behavior have apparently evolved in response to differences in the size and number of gametes produced, which is often amplified by variation in the degree to which a female and male provide parental care for their putative offspring