Knowledge Base Approach and Concept Organisation
Knowledge-Based Approach
Typically our knowledge of a concept is not limited to the concept's features
We usually have some knowledge of the relations among the features
Casual relations
Ahn et al., (2000)
Participants told members of a category tend to have three features
E.g., blurred vision; headaches, insomnia
If one of the 'cause' was missing (blurred vision) in the description, then people did not rate the item as a member of the category
The way we represent a concept changes depending on context (Barsalou, 2008)
If we hear the world frog in isolation, then the phrase "eaten by humans" probably remains inactive, however if you hear the word frog in a French restaurant then the phrase "eaten by humans" may well be activated by the word frog
Yee and Thompson-Schill (2016) argue that there is no core representation activated across all possible contexts
Knowledge-Based Alternative
Allows for several factors that other accounts couldn't explain as having an influence over categorisation
Casual relations
Context
Non-obvious or visible features
Knowledge-based accounts also allow for ad hoc categories
Organisation of Concepts
3 levels of generality
Superordinate
Most general e.g., furniture
Basic
Intermediate level e.g., chair, lamp, table
Subordinate
Specific types of objects e.g., kitchen chair
Conceptual Hierarchies (Rosch et al., 1976)
Ps listed attrivutes of concepts at 3 levels
Superordinate
Few attributes
Lacks informativeness
Basic
Many attributes
Maximal within-category similarity relative to between-category similarity
Balance of informativeness and economy
Subordinate
Many attributes, but similar for complementary categories informativeness good, but lacks economy
Special Properties of Basic Level Concept (Rosch et al., 1976)
Objects are spontaneously named at this level
Acquired first by children
Similar overall shape
Faster recognition compared to other levels
We even tend to use similar motor movements for interacting with category members, for example we tend to physically behave in similar ways with chairs (all chairs) as opposed to tables
Hierarchies of Concepts - Limitations
Not a single level of basic categories - familiarity matters!!!
Tanaka and Taylor (1991) examined the concepts of two groups of people - birdwatchers and dog experts. Showed images of dogs and birds
When dog experts were viewing bird pictures and birdwatchers view dog pictures, both groups used basic level names
When viewing from their expert domain - both groups used subordinate more often
Perceptual distinctiveness
Typically as we have seen we work best at the basic level (dog vs cat) however, if the task involves perceptual distinctiveness then we see an advantage for superordinate categories (e.g., animal or vehicle)
Hierarchical distinctiveness (large categories)
Collins & Loftus (1975) Semantic Networkjou8
Shorter links to connect closely related concepts
Longer links for less closely related subjects
No hierarchical structure; based on person's experience
Assessing Semantic Relatedness
Word association task - participants shown a word and asked to name the first word (or three words) that come to mind
Feature overlay - extent to which different participants use same features to describe
Distance ratings - Ss estimate distance between concepts
Spreading Activation
Activation is the arousal level of a node
Activity spreads out along any link that is connected to an activated node
Concepts that receive activation are primed and more easily accessed from memory
Schemas
Each other in order to create situation models of what happens around us
Event indexing model argues propositions are link based on events being related by time, space, entity, causation and motivation – as parts of events are experiences they are represented overlapping on some of the dimensions (i.e. time or space). Together the events = memory of the situation
Schema – set of related propositions which forms a packet of typical knowledge about the world, events or people
Schema - General knowledge or expectations, based on our relationship with a person, object or event
Memory for common everyday events e.g., restaurant
Schemas guide our recall
Consistency bias
Exaggerate the consistency between our feelings/beliefs
Schema-Based Approach
Bartlett, 1932
Memory uses world knowledge and schemata
Schemata: frameworks for organising information in LTM
At retrieval, Ps reconstruct memories base on their knowledge and schemata
Bartlett rejected the idea of LTM as a warehouse where memory is stored, unchanged until retrieval
Memory is an active and inaccurate process that encode information so as to 'make sure'
Recall became more distorted with each re-telling
People introduced material to make material more rational and coherent, more consistent with their assumptions and existing schema
Memory of stories was reconstructed
Forerunner of more recent work on memory distortions, eye witness memory etc.
Schemas and Incidental Learning
Brewer & Treyans (1981)
Do people show schema-driven memory errors?
Participants asked to wait in 'office' which was a room containing a mixture of schema consistent objects (books, papers etc) and inconsistent objects
Participants recalled more schema-consistent objects than scheme-inconsistent objects present
However, they also falsely recalled more scheme-consistent objects than had actually been present
Why Do We Use Schemas?
Allow us to form expectations
Prevent cognitive overload
Assist with missing information
Assist in visual scene perception
Stereotypes
Stereotypes
Schemas incorporating oversimplified generalisations (often negative) about certain groups
We use them to reduce processing demands
Not fixed - stereotypes (and schemas) can change across time
Garcia - Marques et al, (2006) asked participants to select 5 traits out of 43 that best represented various groups (e.g., immigrants) then again.2 weeks later = considerable instability!
Influenced by context
Semantic Dementia
Patterson et al. (2007) identify semantic dementia as a condition where there is widespread loss of knowledge about the meaning of concepts and words
Different to Alzheimer's where the deficit is primarily with episodic memory
Patients with semantic dementia have problems categorising objects from pictures, difficulty assigning detailed meaning to pictures and difficulty drawing objects
In early stages autobiographical memory spared but by later stages impairment in these too
Knowledge-Based Approach
Typically our knowledge of a concept is not limited to the concept's features
We usually have some knowledge of the relations among the features
Casual relations
Ahn et al., (2000)
Participants told members of a category tend to have three features
E.g., blurred vision; headaches, insomnia
If one of the 'cause' was missing (blurred vision) in the description, then people did not rate the item as a member of the category
The way we represent a concept changes depending on context (Barsalou, 2008)
If we hear the world frog in isolation, then the phrase "eaten by humans" probably remains inactive, however if you hear the word frog in a French restaurant then the phrase "eaten by humans" may well be activated by the word frog
Yee and Thompson-Schill (2016) argue that there is no core representation activated across all possible contexts
Knowledge-Based Alternative
Allows for several factors that other accounts couldn't explain as having an influence over categorisation
Casual relations
Context
Non-obvious or visible features
Knowledge-based accounts also allow for ad hoc categories
Organisation of Concepts
3 levels of generality
Superordinate
Most general e.g., furniture
Basic
Intermediate level e.g., chair, lamp, table
Subordinate
Specific types of objects e.g., kitchen chair
Conceptual Hierarchies (Rosch et al., 1976)
Ps listed attrivutes of concepts at 3 levels
Superordinate
Few attributes
Lacks informativeness
Basic
Many attributes
Maximal within-category similarity relative to between-category similarity
Balance of informativeness and economy
Subordinate
Many attributes, but similar for complementary categories informativeness good, but lacks economy
Special Properties of Basic Level Concept (Rosch et al., 1976)
Objects are spontaneously named at this level
Acquired first by children
Similar overall shape
Faster recognition compared to other levels
We even tend to use similar motor movements for interacting with category members, for example we tend to physically behave in similar ways with chairs (all chairs) as opposed to tables
Hierarchies of Concepts - Limitations
Not a single level of basic categories - familiarity matters!!!
Tanaka and Taylor (1991) examined the concepts of two groups of people - birdwatchers and dog experts. Showed images of dogs and birds
When dog experts were viewing bird pictures and birdwatchers view dog pictures, both groups used basic level names
When viewing from their expert domain - both groups used subordinate more often
Perceptual distinctiveness
Typically as we have seen we work best at the basic level (dog vs cat) however, if the task involves perceptual distinctiveness then we see an advantage for superordinate categories (e.g., animal or vehicle)
Hierarchical distinctiveness (large categories)
Collins & Loftus (1975) Semantic Networkjou8
Shorter links to connect closely related concepts
Longer links for less closely related subjects
No hierarchical structure; based on person's experience
Assessing Semantic Relatedness
Word association task - participants shown a word and asked to name the first word (or three words) that come to mind
Feature overlay - extent to which different participants use same features to describe
Distance ratings - Ss estimate distance between concepts
Spreading Activation
Activation is the arousal level of a node
Activity spreads out along any link that is connected to an activated node
Concepts that receive activation are primed and more easily accessed from memory
Schemas
Each other in order to create situation models of what happens around us
Event indexing model argues propositions are link based on events being related by time, space, entity, causation and motivation – as parts of events are experiences they are represented overlapping on some of the dimensions (i.e. time or space). Together the events = memory of the situation
Schema – set of related propositions which forms a packet of typical knowledge about the world, events or people
Schema - General knowledge or expectations, based on our relationship with a person, object or event
Memory for common everyday events e.g., restaurant
Schemas guide our recall
Consistency bias
Exaggerate the consistency between our feelings/beliefs
Schema-Based Approach
Bartlett, 1932
Memory uses world knowledge and schemata
Schemata: frameworks for organising information in LTM
At retrieval, Ps reconstruct memories base on their knowledge and schemata
Bartlett rejected the idea of LTM as a warehouse where memory is stored, unchanged until retrieval
Memory is an active and inaccurate process that encode information so as to 'make sure'
Recall became more distorted with each re-telling
People introduced material to make material more rational and coherent, more consistent with their assumptions and existing schema
Memory of stories was reconstructed
Forerunner of more recent work on memory distortions, eye witness memory etc.
Schemas and Incidental Learning
Brewer & Treyans (1981)
Do people show schema-driven memory errors?
Participants asked to wait in 'office' which was a room containing a mixture of schema consistent objects (books, papers etc) and inconsistent objects
Participants recalled more schema-consistent objects than scheme-inconsistent objects present
However, they also falsely recalled more scheme-consistent objects than had actually been present
Why Do We Use Schemas?
Allow us to form expectations
Prevent cognitive overload
Assist with missing information
Assist in visual scene perception
Stereotypes
Stereotypes
Schemas incorporating oversimplified generalisations (often negative) about certain groups
We use them to reduce processing demands
Not fixed - stereotypes (and schemas) can change across time
Garcia - Marques et al, (2006) asked participants to select 5 traits out of 43 that best represented various groups (e.g., immigrants) then again.2 weeks later = considerable instability!
Influenced by context
Semantic Dementia
Patterson et al. (2007) identify semantic dementia as a condition where there is widespread loss of knowledge about the meaning of concepts and words
Different to Alzheimer's where the deficit is primarily with episodic memory
Patients with semantic dementia have problems categorising objects from pictures, difficulty assigning detailed meaning to pictures and difficulty drawing objects
In early stages autobiographical memory spared but by later stages impairment in these too