VL

Action, conditioned reflexes, genetics and evolution (Vocabulary Flashcards)

Foundations: Reflexes and the External-stimulus View

  • Skinner’s salamander toe observation: a severed tail still responds to touch, challenging the view that behavior is driven by inner forces (willpower/spirit).

  • Conclusion drawn: behavior can be understood as responses to external stimuli; the stimulus controls the behavior, and the behavior itself is the reflex.

  • Key concept: reflex is an automatic, unlearned response to a specific external stimulus.

  • Implication: many behaviors thought to be under inner control can be explained as reflexes governed by external stimuli.

  • Skinner’s contribution: reflexes can be studied quantitatively; much of an organism’s response to specific stimuli can be predicted.

  • Limitation: reflexes account for only a small portion of the overall behavioral repertoire.

Classical Conditioning: Pavlovian Reflexes

  • Conditioned reflexes extend the idea of reflexes by showing that a neutral stimulus can acquire the ability to elicit a response through association.

  • Process: neutral stimulus paired repeatedly with an unconditioned stimulus; the neutral stimulus becomes conditioned and elicits the reflex without the unconditioned stimulus.

  • Rejection of mentalistic explanations: Pavlov did not claim dogs were thinking of food; conditioning shows the stimulus itself can elicit the reflex.

  • Pavlov’s psychic secretion experiments foundational: demonstrated conditioning and how stimuli come to elicit reflexive responses.

  • Classical conditioning example: the sound commonly perceived as a bell (often depicted as a bell) was not the true auditory stimulus in Pavlov’s experiments; the actual auditory stimulus was a tuning fork. The bell image is a misnomer.

  • Significance: conditioning expands the range of stimuli that can trigger reflexive behaviors, aiding quick, automatic responses to environmental cues.

Conditioning, Adaptation, and Maladaptive Reflexes

  • Not all conditioned reflexes are beneficial; some become maladaptive.

  • Example: fear of flying can be conditioned by increased heart rate and other arousal cues, leading to irrational fear.

  • Mechanism of maladaptive reflex: reflex-like responses persist and can give an illusion of control (e.g., gripping an armrest during turbulence) even when they do not influence the actual event (turbulence).

  • Takeaway: conditioned reflexes have survival value but can overgeneralize or misapply in contexts where they do not serve a useful purpose.

Limits of Conditioning: From Classical to Operant Conditioning

  • Classical conditioning extends the range of stimuli that can elicit reflexive responses but does not generate novel behaviors.

  • As Skinner noted: conditioning increases the set of stimuli that can elicit responses but does not create new behaviors.

  • Novel or complex behaviors require operant conditioning: actions are voluntary, shaped by reinforcement or punishment, and reinforced by their consequences.

  • Distinction: conditioned reflexes (classical conditioning) involve elicited responses to stimuli; operant conditioning involves learning through consequences that shape future behavior.

Genetics, Evolution, and Learning: Phylogeny vs Ontogeny

  • Skinner cautioned against confusing genetic inheritance with learned behavior; similar outcomes can arise from different mechanisms.

  • Phylogeny (evolutionary history): behaviors shaped and passed down across generations through natural selection; reflexes and instincts are typically phylogenetically determined.

  • Ontogeny (lifetime learning): behaviors learned through interaction with the environment, largely via operant conditioning and reinforcement/punishment.

  • Inheritance vs learning: genes provide the foundation (structures and predispositions) but do not directly determine behavior; environment and contingencies shape expression.

  • Moore’s view: genes predispose susceptibility, but do not cause behavior outright; behavior arises from the interaction of genetic endowment and environmental contingencies.

  • Example of phylogeny vs ontogeny: birds migrating (phylogenetic, inherited) vs language acquisition (ontogenetic, learned).

  • Important point: phylogeny and ontogeny interact; language learning is facilitated by phylogenetic predispositions for sound processing and speech production, but actual language use is learned.

  • Implication: do not underestimate the environment; contingencies of reinforcement offer the greatest predictive and control power over behavior.

  • Quote to remember: operant conditioning complements natural selection; genes predispose an individual to be influenced by the environment.

  • Caution against simplistic attributions: just because contingencies aren’t visible doesn’t mean genetics alone explains behavior; avoid using genetic endowment as a default explanation.

  • Personal histories are not excluded from analysis; reinforcement can operate with or without the organism’s explicit awareness.

The Role of Imitation and Neurobiology

  • A key genetic contribution to human behavior: imitation, the capacity to learn by observing others.

  • Imitation can be impaired in certain conditions (e.g., autism); related neural processes may involve mirror neuron systems.

  • Evidence from neuroscience (e.g., mirror neurons) suggests imitation relies on brain networks that can be atypical in autism; environmental manipulation and shaping remain powerful.

  • Practical implication: for children with autism, mirrors and contingent reinforcement can facilitate imitative learning and social skills through operant conditioning and environmental support.

Multilevel Selection: Group, Individual, and Cultural Dynamics

  • David Sloan Wilson (1990) delivered a presidential scholar address at ABAI, advocating group selection or multilevel selection in evolutionary theory.

  • Core idea: genes are transmitted across generations, but organisms and groups act as vehicles for those genes; selection operates at multiple levels.

  • Three levels of selection (Skinner’s framing and Wilson’s framework) help explain complexity:

    • Biogenic (genetic/natural selection): genes shape biological design and are transmitted across generations.

    • Autogenetic (individual learning): operant conditioning and reinforcement shape behavior within an organism’s lifetime.

    • Cultural (cultural selection): social transmission of behaviors, norms, and practices across generations; practices evolve because they benefit the group or society.

  • Interaction among the three levels: biogenic, autogenetic, and cultural selection are interrelated and together influence behavior and survival.

  • Selection by consequences vs cultural selection:

    • Selection by consequences refers to how individual behaviors are reinforced or punished (ontogeny).

    • Cultural selection refers to how behaviors persist or spread through social transmission beyond the individual.

  • Cultural practices (rituals, language, social norms) can evolve because they contribute to group cohesion or survival, even if not immediately advantageous to any single individual.

  • Skinner’s framework provides a basis for understanding how behaviors evolve and are sustained within cultures as well as individuals.

Eugenics, Group Selection, and an Illustrative Case

  • Darwin’s ideas raised concerns about eugenics: selective breeding to improve the human race.

  • Practical concern: how cultural evolution can supersede or complicate strict genetic selection.

  • Purdue hen experiment (1990) as a case study of selection at the group level:

    • Setup: nine hens per cage; the most productive hen from each cage selected to breed the next generation if egg-laying productivity is heritable.

    • Result 1 (gene-centric selection): the line of best egg layers produced a hyper-aggressive strain; many hens were harmed (feather plucking, violence); some cages ended with only three hens.

    • Result 2 (group-level selection): adjusting the method to select the nine hens with the highest overall group productivity to breed the next generation; this approach yielded increased egg production.

  • Takeaway: when traits interact within a social group, group-level selection can produce different outcomes than pure gene-level selection; cultural and group context matters for the success of selected traits.

Contingencies, Reinforcement, and Practical Implications

  • Reinforcement occurs with or without the organism’s knowledge; personal histories contribute to behavior, but reinforcement can still operate implicitly.

  • The environment plays a crucial role in shaping behavior across the lifetime; contingencies of reinforcement provide predictive power for behavior beyond genetic explanations alone.

  • The three-level framework (biogenic, autogenetic, cultural) helps explain how behavior can be shaped by genetics, learning, and social practices in tandem, often across different time scales.

  • Applications in education, therapy, and research:

    • Behavioral analysis can leverage reinforcement schedules to shape adaptive behaviors.

    • Understanding cultural selection informs interventions that target social transmission (e.g., language learning, social norms).

    • Awareness of phylogenetic constraints can guide expectations about innate predispositions and their limits.

  • Ethical and practical implications:

    • Avoid simplistic genetic explanations for complex behaviors; emphasize environmental contingencies and learning history.

    • Recognize that cultural practices can either support or undermine individual well-being; interventions should consider group and cultural context as well as individual learning histories.

Key Formulas and Concepts (LaTeX)

  • Operant behavior as a function of contingencies:
    B = f\left( C \right)
    where B is the operant behavior and C are the contingencies of actions yielding consequences.

  • Complementary view of learning and evolution:
    ext{Operant conditioning} \quad \text{complements} \quad \text{natural selection}

  • Conceptual framing of three levels of selection:

    • Biogenic: genetic/natural selection over generations.

    • Autogenetic: individual learning via reinforcement and punishment.

    • Cultural: social transmission and reinforcement across individuals and generations.

  • Gene-environment interaction (informal representation):
    B = gig(S,E,R\big)
    where S represents genetic predispositions, E represents environmental factors, and R represents reinforcement history.

Takeaways for Exam Preparation

  • Reflexes are automatic, unlearned responses to external stimuli; conditioning extends the range of stimuli but does not create novel behaviors.

  • Classical conditioning demonstrates that neutral stimuli can elicit reflexes through association; Pavlov’s careful control avoids mentalistic explanations.

  • Operant conditioning explains how novel or complex behaviors are formed and maintained via reinforcement and punishment.

  • Phylogeny vs ontogeny: inherited, species-typical behaviors vs learned, lifetime behaviors; both interact to shape behavior.

  • Genes provide a foundation but do not determine behavior outright; environment and contingencies are critical determinants.

  • Imitation and mirror neuron research highlight the role of social learning and neural mechanisms in shaping behavior, with implications for autism and intervention strategies.

  • Skinner’s multilevel framework (biogenic, autogenetic, cultural) describes how behavior is shaped across biological, individual, and social levels, including cultural evolution via social transmission.

  • Cultural selection and group-level selection can yield different outcomes than single-genome selection; the Purdue hen example illustrates consequences of differing selection targets.

  • Always consider contingencies of reinforcement and social context when interpreting behavior; avoid attributing complex behaviors to genetics alone.

  • These concepts have broad real-world relevance for education, therapy, organizational design, and understanding human behavior in social systems.