Key Terms Defined
Flashbulb memory: A vivid, detailed “snapshot” of the moment and context in which emotionally significant news was first heard.
Emotion: Affective arousal that can modulate cognitive processes—here, memory encoding and recall.
Cognitive reliability: How accurately a cognitive process (e.g., memory) reflects the original event.
Context
This study exemplifies the cognitive approach’s use of questionnaire methods to explore how emotion influences memory.
Brown & Kulik proposed a flashbulb memory model, suggesting that surprising, personally relevant events are encoded and stored more vividly.
Outline/Thesis Statement
This essay will evaluate the methods and findings of Brown & Kulik (1977), examine how emotion may enhance memory encoding, and consider the reliability of flashbulb memories.
Theory
Surprising and highly personally relevant events trigger a special memory mechanism, yielding vivid, long-lasting flashbulb memories.
Evidence
Aim: To test whether surprising, emotionally significant events produce flashbulb memories.
Method:
Participants: 80 American males (40 Black, 40 White).
Instrument: Self-report questionnaire asking about the assassination of public figures (e.g., JFK, MLK Jr.) and personal losses.
Questions: Where were you? Who was with you? What were you doing? How did you feel? How important was it? How often have you talked about it?
Findings:
90% of participants recalled rich detail for public-figure deaths.
Detail for personal losses was even higher.
75% of Black participants versus 33% of White participants had flashbulb memories of MLK’s assassination—showing personal relevance matters.
Application
Highlights how emotionally salient events enjoy enhanced encoding and rehearsal, producing more vivid memories.
Informs eyewitness testimony protocols: witnesses may be very confident about emotionally charged events but still be prone to distortion.
Criticism
Sampling bias: Only American males; lacks gender and cultural diversity.
Self-report, retrospective data: Cannot verify actual accuracy; relies on participants’ memories of their own vividness and rehearsal frequency.
No biological measures: Hypothesizes a biological mechanism but does not test it.
No control over rehearsal: Frequency of discussing the event likely varied widely and wasn’t measured objectively.
Demand characteristics/social desirability: Participants may overstate their confidence or detail to conform to perceived expectations.
Unanswered Questions
How do rehearsal and media exposure interact with emotion to shape flashbulb memories?
Would findings generalize to females, other cultures, or non-U.S. contexts?
What is the neurobiological mechanism underpinning enhanced encoding for emotional events?
Practical Use
Guides legal practitioners in evaluating eyewitness confidence versus accuracy.
Suggests educational strategies for leveraging emotional engagement to improve learning and retention.
Counterarguments
Neisser & Harsch (1992) and Talarico & Rubin (2003) found that although people feel very confident in flashbulb memories, their actual accuracy declines similarly to ordinary memories over time.
This suggests that emotion enhances subjective vividness and belief, but not necessarily objective fidelity.
Restate Main Points
Brown & Kulik’s study used questionnaire methods to show that surprise and personal relevance yield vivid, enduring memories.
However, reliance on retrospective self-reports and lack of objective accuracy measures limit claims about true reliability.
Closing Statement
While emotion clearly influences the subjective experience of memory, flashbulb memories may not be immune to distortion—underlining the need to distinguish confidence from accuracy when evaluating emotionally charged recollectio