CC

Scientific Inquiry and Foundations for BIO 201 — Lecture Notes

Overview: science as an iterative process of inquiry

  • Science is a process of inquiry: observe, question, and try to understand the natural world through repeated cycles.
  • Inquiry can operate with or without a predefined hypothesis. In some natural sciences (e.g., oceanographic cruises, long-term monitoring stations), researchers collect large amounts of data first and look for patterns before forming hypotheses.
  • The overarching structure is iterative, not a strictly linear path:
    • Observation → Question → Hypothesis/Prediction → Test → Evaluate data → Decide if the hypothesis is supported or rejected, not proven.
    • Real science often loops back: new observations may lead to new hypotheses, or additional factors must be controlled or considered.
  • Practical implication: this framework helps frame foundational biology (BIO 201) and builds toward the ability to generate testable hypotheses from observations.

The iterative nature of the Scientific Method

  • General structure (not strictly linear):
    • Observation
    • Question
    • Hypothesis or tentative educated guess
    • Prediction (if the hypothesis is true, then this should happen)
    • Test/Experiment
    • Evaluate data
    • Support or reject the hypothesis (never absolutely prove) → may lead to refinement or new hypotheses.
  • When testing, control for one variable at a time to avoid confounding factors.
  • Even simple demonstrations (e.g., a desk lamp) illustrate multiple explanations and the need for controlled testing.

Example: Desk lamp scenario illustrating hypothesis testing

  • Observation: the desk lamp does not turn on.
  • Question: why is the lamp not working?
  • Possible hypotheses:
    • H1: the bulb is burned out.
    • H2: the bulb is not screwed in properly.
  • Prediction for each hypothesis:
    • If H1 is true, then replacing the bulb should fix the lamp.
    • If H2 is true, then screwing the bulb in should fix the lamp.
  • Test: replace the bulb and observe whether the lamp works.
  • Interpretation:
    • Replacing the bulb may make the lamp work, but this does not prove which specific fault existed; there could be other factors (e.g., power surge, wiring issue).
    • The conclusion is: the hypothesis is supported by the data, not definitively proven.
  • Important nuance:
    • If the power to the building surges or drops, the lamp can appear to be fixed by bulb replacement even if the underlying issue wasn’t the bulb.
    • This illustrates that a single data point or test cannot establish absolute truth; more data and controlled testing are needed.

Key concepts: hypotheses, predictions, and control

  • Hypothesis: a tentative, educated guess that can be tested via experimentation.
  • Prediction: a logical consequence of a hypothesis (a statement that can be tested).
  • Control group: a baseline or standard against which results are compared.
  • Control of variables: test one variable at a time to isolate its effect.
  • When experiments generate substantial evidence across many observations, a hypothesis may contribute to building a theory.

Deductive vs Inductive reasoning in science

  • Deductive reasoning (general → specific):
    • Start with a general principle and derive specific conclusions.
    • Example: If the sun always rises in the East (general principle) and you observe the sun in the East, you infer it is morning (specific conclusion).
    • Formalized idea: G
      ightarrow S where G is a general principle and S is a specific outcome.
  • Inductive reasoning (specific → general):
    • Start with many specific observations and generalize to a broader principle.
    • Example: Observing the sun rising in the East on many days leads to the general statement that the sun rises in the East.
    • Formalized idea: from multiple observations S1, S2,
      ots
      , S_n
      ightarrow G where G is a general principle.
  • The point: scientists use both directions of reasoning to make sense of data and to generate/test theories.

The role of facts, hypotheses, and theories in learning biology

  • The course emphasizes not just memorizing facts but developing a framework to form hypotheses when presented with observations.
  • By the end of the semester, students should be able to form plausible hypotheses about novel cell observations based on foundational knowledge.
  • Hypotheses vs Theory:
    • Hypothesis: a testable statement that guides experiments.
    • Theory: a well-supported, broad explanation backed by a large body of evidence from many experiments and researchers.
    • Examples of theories (conceptual sense): the theory of relativity, the theory of evolution.
  • A hypothesis may be supported by many observations; as evidence accumulates, it may contribute to a theory.
  • Important distinction: a theory is not a guess; it is a robust, extensively tested framework.

Why does this matter for scientific thinking in BIO 201?

  • The purpose of learning these processes is to enable flexible thinking when examining cells and biological phenomena.
  • When you see an unusual cell, a solid foundation allows you to generate a plausible hypothesis (e.g., about the cytoskeleton or cell division) and design experiments to gather more information.
  • Case study practice helps you apply general principles to specific observations, bridging theory and experiment.
  • You will learn to avoid overclaiming from a single observation, instead building a coherent set of evidence to support or reject a hypothesis.

The terminology and progression from hypothesis to theory in practice

  • Hypothesis: tentative explanation tested by experiments and observations.
  • When many independent lines of evidence converge, a hypothesis can become a theory.
  • Theories are not proven in an absolute sense; they are supported by cumulative data.
  • Theories may be refined or extended as new data emerge.

Limitations of science: scope, ethics, and practicality

  • Science has limits and cannot answer every kind of question:
    • Not all questions are empirical or testable; some relate to values, beliefs, or morality.
    • Some questions are philosophical or theological and extend beyond the scope of testable hypotheses.
    • Some questions may be unanswerable with current tools or knowledge.
  • Potential biases and limitations:
    • Scientists bring biases and blind spots; replication and collaboration across diverse groups help mitigate this.
    • Bias can affect interpretation and reporting; multiple studies and independent verification improve reliability.
  • Practical and ethical constraints:
    • Some experiments may raise ethical concerns or require prohibitive resources; not all hypotheticals can be tested.
    • Science does not provide direct answers about how to use technology; ethical and societal considerations require human input and discourse.
  • Precision and measurement limits:
    • Some questions require extremely precise measurements; advances in technology improve capabilities but do not guarantee absolute certainty.
  • Mechanistic explanations vs. ultimate why:
    • Science often addresses mechanism (how things work) rather than ultimate purpose or why they exist.
    • Some questions may lie outside empirical testing (e.g., supernatural explanations or belief-based questions).
  • The social nature of science:
    • Science is a collective enterprise, best advanced by diverse researchers and institutions to approach a closer approximation of reality.
    • Reproducibility, peer review, and transparent methods are essential for building credible knowledge.

Connecting to real-world science and classroom practice

  • This framework helps students engage with real-world problems by forming testable hypotheses from observations and designing controlled experiments.
  • Case studies and exercises in BIO 201 will reinforce the practice of hypothesis generation, controlled testing, and interpretation of results.
  • The ultimate goal is to empower students to think critically, reason rigorously, and communicate findings effectively, even when data are imperfect or ambiguous.

Preview: next week

  • The course will delve into the biology of cells, applying these scientific reasoning skills to understanding cell structure and function.
  • Expect to practice identifying hypotheses from observations, designing experiments, and evaluating evidence in the context of cellular biology.