Question: Will intuition and common sense suffice for understanding reality?
Critics argue intuition can be superior, but evidence shows limits.
Examples of intuitive limits:
Quotes illustrating skepticism toward pure intuition:
Overconfidence and hindsight bias:
The risk of relying on common sense: popular ideas (e.g., certain connections between dreams and future events, menstrual phase correlations with emotions) may be wrong; research often overturns common beliefs.
Grandmother intuition isn’t always wrong, but many popular beliefs are not supported by robust data.
Everyday observations are biased by expectancies and selective memory; research tests ideas via controlled methods.
Hindsight examples and statistics:
The role of repetition and replication: over time, persistent replication strengthens confidence in a phenomenon.
Two core ideas introduced early:
Important takeaway: common sense describes what happened more easily than it predicts what will happen; science seeks-testable explanations and predictions.
Core traits: curiosity, skepticism, humility.
Questioning ideas: What do you mean? How do you know? Is the conclusion based on anecdote or evidence? Are alternative explanations possible?
The motto: ‘show me the evidence’ rather than ‘trust my gut’.
The scientist James Randi example: testing extraordinary claims under controlled conditions.
History: science advances by testing competing ideas, not accepting them on authority.
Important virtue: humility; even strong beliefs may be revised in light of evidence. The rat is always right.
Practical view: skeptical, yet open-minded inquiry helps separate science from pseudoscience.
Forceful illustration of skepticism and replication:
The role of replication in science: reproducibility increases confidence in empirical findings.
The scientific attitude in psychology: persistently ask two questions—What do you mean? How do you know?—and demand clear operational definitions to allow replication.
Core idea: science constructs theories that organize observations and imply testable hypotheses.
Theory vs. hypothesis:
Operational definitions: precise statements of the procedures used to define research variables (e.g., intelligence as what an intelligence test measures).
Replication: repeating a study with different participants in different settings to test the generality of the finding.
The process is self-correcting: observations lead to theories, which lead to hypotheses, which are tested and revised.
In testing theories, psychologists use descriptive, correlational, and experimental methods.
A good theory should: (1) organize and link facts, (2) imply testable predictions with practical applications.
Historical and modern references:
Key methodological concepts introduced early:
Case Study:
The Survey:
Naturalistic Observation:
Sampling and representation: importance of representative samples; bias in samples leads to misgeneralization.
Concepts introduced in this section:
Random-sampling illustration: 60 million white beans vs 40 million red beans; a sample of 1500 provides a reliable snapshot of a national population.
Figure and table references (descriptions in text):
Table 1.2: Comparison of research methods (Descriptive, Correlational, Experimental).
Key terms introduced in this section:
Educational takeaway: subliminal perception exists; effects are real but typically small and short-lived; enduring claims are not supported by robust evidence.
Key finding: belief in subliminal benefits can produce a perceived improvement even when no actual effect exists.
Public science literacy: many people incorrectly believe about drug testing and control groups; need to understand placebo effects and experiments.
Summary of research methods and their uses:
")}
Correlation coefficient range and interpretation:
Significance threshold:
Key sample sizes mentioned:
Descriptive statistics:
x_i (arithmetic average).
Example data-related figures you may encounter:
Final takeaway from the chapter: intelligent thinking requires skepticism, humility, and the disciplined use of data and methods to separate sound conclusions from wishful thinking and intuitive but flawed judgments.