Construct Validity (pages 291-299)
Concerns about whether the constructs researchers claim to measure or manipulate are actually being studied.
Emphasis on the need for strong definitions and operational definitions of independent and dependent variables to ensure they accurately represent the constructs.
Good operational definitions ensure that researchers are measuring what they intend to measure.
Statistical Conclusion Validity
Refers to the correct statistical treatment of data and the soundness of the conclusions drawn from those statistics.
Must reflect more than mere random variations; it should allow claims of genuine differences between groups (e.g., strains of rats in a maze).
Examines whether researchers' conclusions about the relationships between independent variables (IV) and dependent variables (DV) are based on appropriate statistical analyses.
Internal Validity
Assesses confidence that a study demonstrates a causal effect between two variables.
Requires three conditions:
Presence of at least two variables (IV and DV).
Establishment of causation.
Clear temporal order of variable changes.
External Validity
Concerned with the extent to which findings can be generalized beyond the study context, across populations and settings.
Replication is critical for validating original findings and enhancing both internal and external validity.
It allows researchers to test findings in different contexts to increase confidence in generalizability.
Ecological Validity
Focuses on how well responses from a research context translate to natural settings.
Involves scientific study of the relationship between living organisms and their environment (Miller, 1988).
Factors include the relationship among the researcher, the participant, and the research situation.
Internal Validity
Represents the ability to make valid inferences about relationships between DV and IV.
The observed effect (measured by the DV) must be caused solely by variations in the IV, avoiding confounding variables.
History
Events occurring during the study that can affect the DV but are not part of the experimental manipulation.
Example: Studying cognitive development and political attitudes during significant historical events, like 9/11, influences perceptions.
Maturation
Natural changes in participants over time can influence the DV.
Example: Increases in intimacy during adolescence regardless of the number of friends.
Testing
Changes in participants' responses due to the measurement process itself.
Example: Monitoring children can lead to altered behaviors during observations.
Instrumentation
Changes in measurement instruments can skew results.
Example: Observers may become more adept at recognizing behaviors over time.
Regression Toward the Mean
Extreme scores on a variable will tend to regress towards the mean over time.
Attrition (Subject Loss)
When participants withdraw, potentially biasing results.
Example: Victimized adolescents drop out of a study over time.
Selection
Pre-existing differences in participant characteristics can mediate results.
Example: Parenting programs where parents self-select into conditions.
Diffusion of Treatment
Participants communicate and reduce differences between experimental groups.
Random Selection
Ensures everyone in the participant pool has an equal chance of being selected, enhancing generalizability.
Elimination Procedure: Remove specific groups that may introduce bias.
Equating Procedure: Assign similar numbers of individuals to different conditions.
Counterbalancing Procedure: Change the order of conditions to control for order effects.
Most powerful for eliminating confounding variables, ensuring controls are in place through random assignment.
Random sampling vs. random assignment: Sampling refers to selecting a representative sample, while assignment places those samples into specific experimental conditions.