Lab Reports: Background Research, Hypothesis, and Experimental Design Notes

Background Research and Introduction in Lab Reports

  • In your lab reports, begin with background research to set the context for your problem.

  • Do not jump in simply because you’re interested; establish what others have done before you.

  • Use this prior work as the basis for your introduction and cite it as references.

  • References should include original research or clear definitions related to your topic.

Hypothesis and Predictions

  • Hypothesis is a belief about a phenomenon or observation, a tentative explanation, or an educated guess answering your question.

  • Key idea: it should be an educated guess, not a guess without basis.

  • Hypotheses should be derived from a literature search or systematic observations; if there is no literature, present systematic observations of the phenomenon.

  • Predictions are testable and precise; vague predictions lead to lost points.

  • If you know something to be true from literature, formalize it as a hypothesis and design tests for it.

  • Example hypotheses from the transcript:

    • "If heat helps clones grow" (hypothesis derived from literature or prior observation).

    • "Heat must be beneficial to plants" (another explicit hypothesis).

  • When forming hypotheses, explain where your intuition comes from (the literature or prior observations).

  • Avoid changing your hypothesis after seeing results; this is biased and bad science.

Literature Search and Use of References

  • Compile a literature backend: look up original research or definitions to support context.

  • The success or failure of prior studies should inform your approach, but you must present data and methods transparently regardless of outcome.

  • Example from the transcript: a study on probiotics in mice showed that encapsulating probiotics in certain materials actually increased inflammation; despite the negative conclusion, the data and methods were presented in detail, illustrating the value of reporting all results.

Experimental Design: Variables, Controls, and Treatments

  • Experimental design involves planning how you will test your hypothesis while accounting for variables.

  • Key variable roles:

    • Independent variable: what you deliberately change (e.g., fertilizer concentration, temperature).

    • Dependent variable: what you measure (e.g., plant growth, biomass).

    • Control variables: factors kept constant to prevent confounding.

  • Control group: essential in most experiments; provides a baseline for comparison.

  • Example setup described in the transcript:

    • Treatments: 5% fertilizer group, 10% fertilizer group, and a control group with 0% fertilizer.

    • Group sizes: aim for the same size across groups (e.g., 20 plants per group).

    • Possible confounding factor example: temperature differences (e.g., 20°C for the 5% group, 15°C for the 10% group, 0°C for the control) could influence results if temperature is not controlled.

    • If you’re only studying fertilizer concentration, you must keep other variables (like temperature) as controls to avoid confounding.

  • Experimental design components:

    • Multiple treatment groups with clear, fixed levels of the independent variable.

    • Explicit, fixed control conditions.

    • Transparent description of how the independent and dependent variables are measured.

Replication, Sample Size, and Reliability

  • To ensure results are reliable and not due to chance, replicate the experiment.

  • The transcript emphasizes repeating the experiment in multiple trials:

    • Week 1: run with the same control variables and dependent variables.

    • Week 2: repeat with the same conditions.

    • Week 3: repeat again.

    • This repetition yields the notion of n = 3 replicates, i.e., three independent runs.

  • Rationale: replication helps confirm that results are consistent and not random.

  • Terminology:

    • Replicates: independent repetitions of the experiment under the same conditions.

    • Sample size: number of units per treatment group (e.g., 20 plants per group).

    • n = 3 in the example refers to the number of experimental runs (replicates).

Example Experimental Scenario: Fertilizer Concentration and Temperature

  • Experimental design details from the transcript:

    • Independent variable: fertilizer concentration with three levels: 5%, 10%, and 0% (control).

    • Dependent variable: plant growth measure (not explicitly named in the transcript; typically height, biomass, etc.).

    • Group sizes: 20 plants per treatment group.

    • Temperature as a potential confounder: 5% group at 20°C, 10% group at 15°C, control at 0°C.

    • Purpose of controls: ensure that observed effects are due to fertilizer concentration, not temperature or other factors.

  • Overall design approach:

    • Use three groups with equal sizes to facilitate comparison.

    • Keep all variables other than the independent variable as constant as possible or explicitly tested.

    • If temperature differences are present, acknowledge their potential impact and control in future iterations.

Data Collection, Analysis, and When to Repeat

  • After collecting results, analyze data to determine whether the observed effects align with the hypotheses and predictions.

  • Emphasize that replication improves reliability and validity of findings: consistent results across trials imply data are robust.

  • If a confounding factor is suspected, discuss how it might have influenced results and how you would control it in future iterations.

Experimental Design: Terminology and Concepts

  • Experimental design is the overarching process of planning experiments to test hypotheses with careful control of variables.

  • Core elements of the design:

    • Independent variable(s) and its levels.

    • Dependent variable(s) to be measured.

    • Control variables to minimize bias and confounding.

    • Treatment groups and control group definitions.

    • Sample size per group and the number of replicates (n).

  • In this course, students will design their own experiments as part of the curriculum, reinforcing these concepts.

Discussion and Conclusion

  • The discussion section connects your results to the broader literature.

  • You compare your data with what others found, noting agreements or discrepancies.

  • You discuss limitations, potential biases, and the implications of your results.

  • The conclusion should summarize findings, relate back to the initial research question, and indicate possible future work.

  • Emotional tone or personal reflections (as suggested by the transcript) are not part of the formal methods; focus on data-driven interpretation.

Ethical, Philosophical, and Practical Implications

  • Ethical implication: presenting all results, including negative or null findings, with complete透明 and transparent methods.

  • Practical implication: rigorous experimental design improves reproducibility and reliability of scientific conclusions.

  • Philosophical implication: science builds on prior work; overconfidence in unvalidated hypotheses without literature support is a risk.

Quick Reference: Key Terms and Concepts

  • Background research: literature review and context setting for a study.

  • Introduction: context and rationale based on the background research.

  • Hypothesis: an educated, testable guess derived from prior work or observations.

  • Prediction: a specific, testable statement derived from the hypothesis.

  • Independent variable: the variable you change deliberately.

  • Dependent variable: the variable you measure.

  • Control variable: factors kept constant to avoid confounding.

  • Control group: a baseline group lacking the treatment.

  • Replicates: independent repetitions of an experiment under the same conditions.

  • Sample size: number of experimental units per treatment group (e.g., 20 plants).

  • n: number of replicates; in the example, n = 3.

  • Experimental design: the overall plan for testing hypotheses with control of variables and replication.

  • Confounding factor: an uncontrolled variable that can influence the outcome (e.g., temperature differences).

  • Data analysis: interpreting results to assess hypothesis support and compare with literature.

Notable examples and explicit figures from the transcript

  • Fertilizer concentration levels: 5 ext{%}, 10 ext{%}, 0 ext{% (control)}

  • Group sizes: 20 plants per group

  • Temperature settings used in the example: 20^ ext{°C} for the 5% group, 15^ ext{°C} for the 10% group, and 0^ ext{°C} for the control

  • Replication concept: n = 3 (three independent runs)

  • Emphasis on keeping a clear and precise hypothesis, and having testable predictions