psychology 9/10Notes on Correlation, Causation, and Experimental Design

Variables and measurement

  • A variable is anything that can be measured in psychology. Examples discussed: eye color (a variable), love (a variable), having thoughts about stress (a variable).
  • In research, two big terms are correlation and causation:
    • Correlation: a relationship or connection between two variables. As one variable changes, the other tends to change in a related way (positive or negative).
    • Causation: a cause-and-effect relationship where one variable directly influences the other.
  • The phrase you’ll hear a lot: correlation does not equal causation. Just because two variables are correlated does not mean one causes the other.
    • Example from the transcript: ice cream sales ↑ in summer and crime rates ↑ in summer. The correlation exists, but ice cream sales do not cause crime; both rise due to hotter weather and more outdoor activity.
  • Why study correlations? They help us predict outcomes and plan actions (e.g., predicting budget needs when time with a partner increases, or adjusting patrols when crime tends to rise in the summer).
  • Quick practical tip from lecture: correlation coefficients quantify strength and direction of relationships; the closer the coefficient is to +1 or -1, the stronger the relationship; closer to 0 means weaker relationship.

Correlation coefficients and interpretation

  • The mathematical symbol for a correlation is r; in psychology reports you’ll see statements like r = -0.89 or r = +0.75.
  • The range of r is from -1 to +1.
  • Strength and direction:
    • Positive correlation: as one variable increases, the other also increases; or as one decreases, the other also decreases.
    • Negative correlation: as one variable increases, the other decreases.
    • If one variable increases while the other decreases (e.g., time with a partner increases, money decreases), that’s a negative correlation.
  • Examples discussed:
    • Positive: more positive feedback ↗ higher confidence.
    • Negative: less time spent at the gym ↘ less muscle mass (a positive correlation if you consider both decreasing to be a positive correlation since they move together in the same direction).
  • Example values mentioned:
    • Very weak/near zero: r = -0.001 (negative but extremely small, effectively no relationship).
    • Moderate to strong positive: r = +0.75 (strong positive relationship).
    • Strong negative: r ≈ -0.89 (strong negative relationship; the exact sign context depends on how the variables move).
  • Quick math intuition:
    • Positive × Positive = Positive (e.g., both ↑ or both ↓).
    • Negative × Negative = Positive (two negatives give a positive relationship).
    • Positive × Negative = Negative (one up, the other down).
  • Reminder: r is the standard symbol for the correlational coefficient in psychology reports.

Confounding variables

  • A confounding variable is a third variable that can influence the observed relationship between the two studied variables, potentially biasing the strength or even the direction.
  • Examples from discussion:
    • Ice cream sales and crime rates: confounded by summertime/hot weather.
    • Time with a partner and money: other factors like work hours and coffee purchases can influence money, not just relationship time.
  • Why it matters: confounds can make a correlational relationship appear stronger or weaker than it truly is.

Confirmation bias

  • Defined: the tendency to pay more attention to information that supports your beliefs and to disregard information that contradicts them.
  • Why it matters in psychology: it can skew interpretation of data, lead to self-fulfilling prophecies, or biased conclusions.
  • Examples from the lecture:
    • Personal stories about truck drivers in Texas (initial belief that truck drivers are rude) and how expectancies can color interpretation of behavior.
    • A roommate’s belief about a dating partner influencing how they interpreted that person’s actions.
    • Monstrous fear example (monsters under the bed) and placebo-based reassurance (monster spray) as a demonstration of belief shaping experience.
  • Related concept mentioned: observer bias was touched on, but the main focus here is confirmation bias as a general cognitive bias.
  • Video prompt shown to illustrate confirmation bias and its consequences (including potential self-fulfilling prophecies and ethical concerns).

Experiments: designing to establish causation

  • The central claim: experiments are the only way to prove causation (a cause-and-effect relationship).
  • Steps to set up an experiment:
    1) Develop a specific, testable hypothesis (often based on observation; relates to inductive/deductive reasoning).
    2) Assign operational definitions to the variables (precise, measurable definitions so others can replicate).
    3) Form two groups: experimental and control. Participants are the people in the study. The experimental group is exposed to the manipulated variable; the control group is not.
    4) Consider experimenter bias: the researcher’s expectations, experiences, or beliefs might influence interpretation of observations.
    5) Distinguish between confirmation bias (a broad, general bias) and experimenter bias (specific to conducting experiments).
    6) Understand randomization: randomly assign participants to control or experimental groups to reduce systematic differences between groups.
    7) Ethical considerations: informed consent, potential deception, debriefing, and animal research ethics.
  • Key terms:
    • Hypothesis: testable statement about a relationship between variables.
    • Operational definitions: precise definitions of variables to be measured.
    • Participants: people in the study.
    • Experimental group: receives the manipulated variable.
    • Control group: does not receive the manipulation.
    • Independent variable (IV): the variable the researcher manipulates or changes.
    • Dependent variable (DV): the variable measured to observe the effect.
    • Confounding variables: external factors that may influence the DV or the IV and must be controlled or acknowledged.
  • Example scenario walkthroughs from the lecture:
    • Ice cream consumption and life satisfaction: IV = ice cream consumption frequency; DV = life satisfaction. Control group = no ice cream; Experimental group = defined ice cream intake (e.g., one cone per day). The discussion emphasized the need for precise operational definitions (e.g., type of ice cream, brand, portion size, etc.).
    • Sleep and patience in parents: IV = amount of sleep; DV = patience level (operationalized via Likert scale, body language cues, heart rate). Randomly assign groups to normal sleep vs four hours sleep; discuss potential measurement challenges.
    • Teenagers and screen time vs anxiety: IV = screen time (>4 hours daily vs 0 hours); DV = anxiety (operationalized via Likert scale or a standard anxiety measure).
    • Toy size and enjoyment in children: IV = toy size (e.g., 12-inch vs. larger; control often the smaller size in two-size scenarios); DV = enjoyment (operationalized via duration of interaction, play behavior, or explicit ratings).
    • Monday vs Friday memory: IV = day of week (manipulated by testing on Monday vs Friday); DV = memory/recall (operationalized via quizzes).
  • Types of designs:
    • Single-blind study: participants do not know if they’re in the control or experimental group.
    • Double-blind study: neither participants nor the researchers interacting with them know which group participants are in.
    • Placebo effect: participants’ expectations can produce real changes in experience, even when the treatment is inert.
  • Placebo effect in depth:
    • Classic example: a fake painkiller made participants report less pain.
    • Historical context: placebos were used to test treatments; ethical concerns have reduced their use in some contexts.
    • Modern view: placebos can confound results and raise ethical questions about deception; sometimes they serve as a control to compare new vs. old/alternative treatments.
  • Debriefing and deception in experiments:
    • Informed consent is required; deception is allowed if it does not harm participants and if participants are debriefed afterwards.
    • Debriefing explains the true nature of the study and addresses any short- or long-term effects.
  • Reliability and validity:
    • Reliability: consistency of results across repeated trials or different samples.
    • Validity: accuracy of the measurements—whether the study actually measures what it intends to measure.
    • Example: using a ruler to measure eye color would be neither a valid approach nor an appropriate measure for color—a validity issue.
  • Ethics in research:
    • Informed consent: participants must be told what the study is about and agree to participate in writing.
    • Deception and liar consent: allowed in some cases if non-harmful, with debriefing afterward.
    • Debriefing: explains the study’s purpose and potential side effects or risks after participation.
    • Animal research: tightly regulated, with review boards to protect animals; more oversight than some human studies because animals cannot consent.
  • Quasi-experiments:
    • When a true experiment is not possible (e.g., you cannot ethically assign biological sex, race, etc.), researchers may conduct quasi-experiments.

Practice and group activity insights from the lecture

  • The instructor had six hypotheses and students were grouped to identify:
    • The two variables under study (IV and DV).
    • Operational definitions for those variables.
    • Which variable is independent and which is dependent.
    • Who is in the control vs the experimental group.
  • Example from the session: Parents’ sleep and patience; Teenagers’ screen time and anxiety; Big toys vs small toys; Day of the week and memory.
  • Emphasis on being specific in operational definitions: the more precise, the easier replication by other researchers.
  • Emphasis on random sampling as a way to reduce systematic differences between groups, and the idea that ethical considerations may limit certain manipulations (leading to quasi-experiments).

Real-world relevance and takeaways

  • Correlation helps with prediction and planning but cannot establish causation by itself.
  • Experimental design is required to claim causation, with careful attention to operational definitions, randomization, and control conditions.
  • Biases (confirmation bias, experimenter bias) can distort interpretation; strategies like single/double-blind designs and placebo controls help mitigate bias.
  • Ethics are central: informed consent, deception safeguards, debriefing, and consideration of animal welfare.
  • Reliability and validity are foundational for trustworthy research; researchers must choose measurement tools and procedures that maximize both.
  • The role of confounding variables reminds us that observed relationships may be due to multiple interacting factors; careful design and analysis are needed to draw valid conclusions.

Quick recap: key terms to remember

  • Variable: anything that can be measured.
  • Correlation: relationship between two variables (association, not causation).
  • Causation: one variable causes the other.
  • Correlation coefficient: symbolized as r, range \-1 to 1; strength increases as |r| approaches 1.
  • Independent variable (IV): the manipulated variable.
  • Dependent variable (DV): the measured outcome.
  • Operational definition: precise, replicable definition of a variable.
  • Confounding variable: third variable that distorts the observed relationship.
  • Placebo effect: improvement due to expectations; not due to the treatment itself.
  • Single-blind / Double-blind: methods to reduce bias by concealing group assignment.
  • Reliability: consistency of results.
  • Validity: accuracy of what is being measured.
  • Informed consent / Debriefing / Deception: ethical considerations in experiments.
  • Quasi-experiment: a study that resembles an experiment but lacks random assignment or manipulation of the IV.
  • Random sampling / Random assignment: strategies to reduce bias and achieve representative groups.
  • Ethics in animal research: rigorous regulatory oversight.