P

Mind Over Milkshakes: Detailed Notes

Mind Over Milkshakes: Mindsets, Not Just Nutrients, Determine Ghrelin Response

Objective

The study aimed to determine if physiological satiation, measured by ghrelin levels, varies depending on the mindset towards food consumption.

Methods

  • Participants: 46 participants were involved in the study.
  • Procedure: Participants consumed a 380-calorie milkshake on two separate occasions.
    • They were misled to believe it was either a 620-calorie "indulgent" shake or a 140-calorie "sensible" shake.
  • Measurements: Ghrelin levels were measured through intravenous blood samples at three intervals:
    • Baseline (20 min)
    • Anticipatory (60 min)
    • Post-consumption (90 min)
  • During the first interval (20-60 min), participants viewed and rated the misleading label of the shake.
  • During the second interval (60-90 min), they consumed and rated the milkshake.

Results

  • Ghrelin Response:
    • The "indulgent" mindset led to a significantly steeper decline in ghrelin levels after consumption.
    • The "sensible" mindset resulted in a relatively flat ghrelin response.
  • Satiety: Participants' satiety aligned with their beliefs about what they consumed, rather than the actual nutritional content.

Conclusions

  • The impact of food consumption on ghrelin may be psychologically influenced.
  • Mindset significantly affects physiological responses to food.

Keywords

Mindset, nutrition, ghrelin, hunger, product labeling

Introduction

  • Mindset (thoughts, beliefs, and expectations) is a crucial component in health.
  • Expectation to heal enhances medication effects.
  • Interpretation of events influences the impact of stress and illness.
  • Identifying housework as exercise elicits physiological benefits.
  • Mind and body are interdependent.

Food Expectancies

  • Mindset determines taste and preference.
  • Examples:
    • Coke tastes better from a brand-name cup (McClure et al., 2004).
    • Strawberry yogurt and cheese spreads are less enjoyable when labeled "low-fat" (Wardle & Solomons, 1994).
    • Adding vinegar to beer as a "special ingredient" can improve taste ratings if the consumer is unaware of the vinegar (Lee, Frederick, & Ariely, 2006).
    • Perceived cost of wine affects the brain's pleasure center (Plassmann, O’Doherty, Shiv, & Rangel, 2008).

Food Labels and Perceptions

  • Affect hunger and subsequent food consumption.
  • People adjust eating based on perceived calorie intake.
  • High calorie preload leads to greater fullness and less consumption.
  • Low calorie preload leads to more hunger and increased consumption.
  • Restrained eating can moderate these differences.
  • Counterregulatory eating: Highly restrained participants eat more after perceiving a high calorie preload.

Ghrelin and Energy Balance

  • Ghrelin is a gut peptide identified as an essential indicator of energy insufficiency.
  • When energy intake is low, ghrelin is secreted, leading to hunger and motivating consumption.
  • As energy intake increases, ghrelin levels are suppressed, reducing appetite and increasing satiety (Baynes, Dhillo, & Bloom, 2006; Murphy, Dhillo, & Bloom, 2006).
  • Ghrelin levels should rise and fall in proportion to calories consumed for metabolic balance (Zigman & Elmquist, 2003).
  • Complex communication exists between metabolic and neurological systems.
  • Abnormalities in ghrelin signaling may be associated with weight gain and obesity (Cummings, 2006).
  • Ghrelin administration promotes food intake and body weight gain (Theander-Carillo et al., 2006; Wren et al., 2001, as cited in Castaneda et al., 2010).

Current Study

  • Investigated whether mindset influences ghrelin release in response to food consumption.
  • Considers the moderating influence of restraint on eating behavior and satiety (Heatherton, Polivy, & Herman, 1989).
  • Analyzed the influence of restrained eating on ghrelin response (Schur, Cummings, Callahan, & Foster-Schubert, 2008).

Method Details

  • Participants: Recruited through flyers for a "Shake Tasting Study" at the Yale Center for Clinical Investigation.
    • Received $75 for two 2.5-hour sessions.
    • Ages 18-35, BMI in normal to overweight range (M = 22.5, SD = 4.04).
    • Prescreened for diabetes, pregnancy, chronic conditions, and food allergies.
    • Final Data: 46 participants (65% women, 78% student, 22% community member; 56% White, 12% African American, 11% Asian American, 10% Hispanic/Latino, 11% other).
  • Design and Procedure:
    • Two 2.5-hour sessions, 1 week apart, at 8:00 a.m. or 8:20 a.m. after an overnight fast.
    • Participants were told the study evaluated whether two milkshakes with different nutrient contents tasted similar and examined the body’s reaction to nutrients (high vs. low fat/sugar).
    • Milkshakes were identical in content but had different labels: "indulgent" (high fat, high calorie) or "sensible" (low fat, low calorie).
    • An intravenous catheter was placed for blood drawing.
    • Blood samples were drawn at 20 min (baseline), 60 min (anticipatory), and 90 min (post-consumption).
    • Participants viewed and rated the label (20-60 min) and then consumed and rated the milkshake (60-90 min).
    • Shake consumption was to be completed within the first 10 minutes of the interval.
    • Order of milkshake presentation was counterbalanced.

Measures

  • Ghrelin: Measured using a double antibody RIA (GHRT-89HK) with intra-assay variability of 4-10% and inter-assay variability of 4.8-12.8% (Millipore; St. Charles, MO).
    • Samples kept on ice, spun, and plasma stored at -70 °C until analysis.
    • Total blood collected: 90 ccs (45 ccs per visit).
  • Taste Ratings: Participants commented on smell, appearance, taste, enjoyment, and healthiness via 100-mm visual analogue scales (0 = not at all, 100 = extremely).
  • Hunger Ratings: Subjective feelings of hunger were rated 10 minutes before each ghrelin measurement using 100-mm visual analogue scales (0 = not at all, 100 = extremely).
  • Restrained Eating: The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van Strien, Frijters, Bergers, & Defares, 1986) was used.
    • Only the restraint subscale was analyzed.
    • This scale assesses unidimensional restraint.
    • Reliability in the current sample: Cronbach’s α = .82. Restrained eating was dichotomized at the midpoint.

Results Details

  • Perceived Healthiness and Tastiness: Mixed-model ANOVA was conducted with shake type (indulgent, sensi-shake), restrained eating (nonrestrained, restrained), and order (Session 1, Session 2) as factors.
    • Perceived Healthiness: Significant main effect for type of shake, F(1, 89) = 42.50, p < .01, \eta^2 = .33. Sensi-shake was rated as healthier than the indulgent shake, t(44) = 15.61, p < .01.
    • Perceived Tastiness: No significant main or interaction effects.
  • Ghrelin and Hunger: Mixed-model ANOVA with time (baseline, anticipatory, postconsumption), shake type, & order.
    • No significant main effects or interactions involving order, so data was collapsed over order.
    • 2 (shake type) x 3 (time) repeated-measures GLM with restrained eating.
    • Quadratic shake x time interaction effect expected.
    • Ghrelin: Reliable quadratic effect, F(1, 44) = 4.36, p < .04, \eta^2 = .091. Steeper rise in ghrelin in anticipation of the indulgent shake, followed by a steeper reduction after consumption.
    • Indulgent mindset: Moderate physiological craving followed by significant satiety.
    • Sensible mindset: Flat or slightly increased ghrelin levels.
    • No significant interaction or between-subjects effect of restrained eating.
    • The primary driver of the quadratic effect was the response to consuming the shake, F(1, 44) = 5.75, p < .02, \eta^2 = .12.
    • The anticipation effect was not significant, F(1, 44) = 0.94, p = .34, \eta^2 = .02.
    • Hunger: No significant main or interaction effects.

Discussion

  • Participants had a steeper decline in ghrelin after consuming the indulgent shake compared to the sensible shake.
  • Ghrelin response was dependent on perceived expectancies rather than objective nutritional differences.
  • Psychological mindset of sensibility while eating may dampen the effect of ghrelin.
  • Elevated ghrelin levels can cause increased body weight and fat gain (Murphy et al., 2006; Zigman & Elmquist, 2003).
  • Ghrelin antagonists can have the opposite effect (Castaneda et al., 2010).
  • Flat ghrelin profiles in response to the sensible shake may lead to increased appetite and decreased metabolism.

Limitations and Future Research

  • No significant differences in subjective hunger were found, possibly due to measurement timing or method.
  • Additional research is needed to understand the relationship between ghrelin levels, subjective hunger, and subsequent consumption.
  • Future studies should incorporate subsequent consumption and consider restrained eating.
  • Label manipulation is common, and inaccurate or misleading health claims may be dangerous.
  • The juxtaposition of unhealthy nutrients with healthy proclamations might lead to inadequate ghrelin suppression.
  • Mindset alterations have the potential to elicit a seemingly inappropriate sense of satiation.
  • Further research is warranted to understand better how psychological factors influence the biological impact of food.