Module 6: Vision and the Brain

Properties of Light

Visible Light is a narrow band of the spectrum of electromagnetic radiation that receptors in our eyes detect

Three Dimensions determine the perceived color:

  1. Hue: dominant wavelength

  2. Saturation: purity

  3. Brightness: intensity

Anatomy of the Eye

  • Sensory information from the environment (e.g., light) is detected by specialised neurons called sensory receptors. The information detected is transformed into changes in the electrical potential of neurons (i.e., the way neurons talk to one another) via a process of sensory transduction. The changes in neuron charge generated by sensory information are called receptor potentials.

  • The sensory receptors for light information are called photoreceptors which are located in the retina of the eye.

Retina

  • The retina is located at the back of they eye and is part of the brain

  • Light must be focused on the retina for vision because it contains billions of sensory receptors called photoreceptors, which collect information and send it to the CNS via the optic nerve

Focusing Light on the Retina

  • Extraocular muscles hold our eyes in place and move them

  • The muscles are attached to the sclera - the white outer coating of most of the eye

  • The sclera is opaque and does not let the light enter

  • The cornea is the transparent part of the eye at the front

  • The amount of light hitting the retina is determined by the pupil size

  • The lens is behind the iris, it changes shape to help focus light on the retina via the process of accomodation

    • Focusing on a far point makes the lens narrower, wider when it a near point.

The Retina and Sensory Transduction

The retina has three cellular levels: photoreceptor cells, bipolar cells, and ganglion cells

Photoreceptors

Two types:

  • Rods (92 mil)

    • Most prevalent in the peripheral retina, not found in the fovea

    • Sensitive to low levels of light

    • Provide only monochromatic information

    • Poor acuity

  • Cones (4.6 mil)

    • Most prevalent in the central retina, found in fovea

    • Sensitive to moderate to high levels of light

    • Provide information about hue

    • Provide excellent acuity

  • The Fovea is the central region of the retina very important for visual acuity (fine spatial detail)

  • Cones are found in the fovea, and predominate the central retina

  • Rods are NOT found in the fovea and predominate the peripheral retina

Blind Spot

  • The blind spot occurs is because of the optic disk - the place where the axons of neurons sending information onwards to the cortex (and other places!) gather together and leave the eye

  • There are no photoreceptor cells at the optic disk so there is no way for light to be detected

The Layers of Cells

  • Photoreceptor cells form synapses with Bipolar Cells, which then form synapses with Ganglion Cells

  • The axons of ganglion cells converge to form the Optic Nerve

Visual Transduction and Receptive Fields in the Retina

  • Transduction is the process by which environmental energy is converted into a change in a neuron's membrane potential.

  • In the visual system, transduction occurs in photoreceptors in the retina.

  • Photopigments are molecules in photoreceptors made of:

    • A protein (opsin)

    • A lipid (retinal)

  • When exposed to light, opsin and retinal separate, triggering intracellular events that change the photoreceptor’s membrane potential.

  • The arrangement of rods and cones affects the receptive field of ganglion cells.

  • The number of photoreceptors per ganglion cell differs between central and peripheral vision:

    • In the peripheral retina, many photoreceptors connect to one ganglion cell → large receptive fieldslow visual acuity

    • In the central fovea, one cone connects to one ganglion cell → small receptive fieldshigh visual acuity

  • High acuity in the fovea allows for fine visual detail, such as reading or recognizing faces.

  • To see detail, humans must move their eyes to bring objects into the fovea.

  • Example: When reading, our eyes focus on words to clearly process letters for comprehension.

Central and Peripheral Vision: Eye Movements

Because peripheral vision is blurry we want the light to project onto the fovea for detailed processing of relevant information, so we move our eyes

Vergence movements

Eyes rotating inwards and outwards depending on the distance from the object of focus

Saccade movements

Rapid jerky movements of the eyes used when we scan a scene

Pursuit movements

Steady and smooth eye movements following a moving object

The Visual Pathway

  • The axons of ganglion cells in the retina converge to form the optic nerve

  • The optic nerves from each eye meet at the base of the brain to form the optic chiasm

  • Information is then sent on to the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) of the thalamus

  • Then information travels to the primary visual cortex (V1, striate cortex)

  • Then information goes to the visual association cortex (V2, extrastriate cortex)

  • The ganglion cell axons from the inner halves of the retina cross the chiasm and go to the LGN on the opposite side of the brain; but axons from the outer halves of the retina go to the LGN on the respective side

  • Thus, each hemisphere of the brain receives information from the opposite side of the visual field (contralateral)

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

LGN is layered, some layers receive input from ganglion cells of the contralateral eye, some from the ipsilateral eye

  • Magnocellular layers: two inner layers

    • Relay information to the visual cortex that is important for the perception of form, movement, depth, and small differences in contrast (larger receptive fields)

  • Parvocellular layers: four outer layers

    • Relay information to the visual cortex important for colour perception and the processing of fine spatial detail (similar receptive fields)

  • Koniocellular sublayers: beneath other two types

    • Relay information to the visual cortex important for processing blue light (colour)

Striate Cortex

After processing in LGN information is passed on to the primary (striate, V1) visual cortex

  • V1 is the first cortical region that combines information from several sources to detect visual features bigger than the receptive fields of ganglion and LNG cells

    • Highly structured + has layers like LGN

    • The striate cortex of one hemisphere of the brain contains a map of the contralateral half of the visual field = topographic mapping.

      • Has 12 segments of the visual fields assigned a personal part of the cortex

    • Processing from the fovea utilises four of the segments (1-4)

Extrastriate Cortex

The Striate Cortex cannot perceive entire objects or scenes and only processes basic visual features. For more complex information it sends information to the Extrastriate Cortex

  • Extrastriate Cortex (V2–V5)

    • Contains specialised regions, each with a topographic map of the visual field

    • Neurons are tuned to specific types of visual information (e.g., V4 = colour, V5/MT = motion)

    • Organised hierarchically: information flows from areas closer to V1 to higher-level regions for advanced processing

    Dorsal Stream ("Where" Pathway)

    • Processes object location, movement, speed, and direction

    • Supports spatial awareness and action guidance

    Ventral Stream ("What" Pathway)

    • Processes object identity and colour

    • Supports object recognition and meaning

Colour Perception

Different cells in the visual system are specialised to process different types of information.  This specialisation is certainly important when it comes to colour perception.

Colour Vision and the Retina

In terms of photoreceptors, the responsibility of processing colour falls onto cones, rather than rods.

The retina detects different colours because it contains three types of cones, each sensitive to different hues, which is referred to as Trichromatic Coding (blue, green, and red)

How does it work?

  • The different types of cones are sensitive to different wavelengths due to them having different absorption characteristics

  • Absorption is determined by the opsin (protein) in photopigments - specifically different opsins absorb different wavelengths

  • Therefore, trichromatic coding can explain many differences in colour vision

Colour Vision Differences

  • Protanopia: confuse red and green and see the world in shades of blue and yellow; is thought to be due to red cones, in individuals with such condition, being filled with green cone opsin

  • Deuteranopia: also confuse red and green; thought to occur because green cones are filled with red cone opsin

  • Tritanopia: see the world in greens and reds; retina lacks blue cones

  • Monochromatic: do not perceive hue differences; retina lacks all three cones

Colour Processing in Ganglion Cells

  • Ganglion cells use an opponent colour system

  • Ganglion cells respond to pairs of primary colours (red vs green, yellow vs blue)

  • Therefore, the brain has red-green ganglion cells, and yellow-blue ganglion cells

Each ganglion cell is excited by one colour in the pair and inhibited by the other. For example:

  • A red-green opponent cell increases firing for red (excitatory) and decreases firing for green (inhibitory).

  • A yellow-blue opponent cell decreases firing for blue (inhibitory)

  • Yellow light causes both excitatory and inhibitory signals to be sent to the red-green cell, because it activates both types of cones, which cancels out the signal.

  • At the same time, yellow light causes red and green cones to send excitatory messages to the yellow-blue ganglion cells, which causes it to increase firing signalising yellow.

This system helps the brain detect colour contrast and prevents us from seeing “impossible” colours like reddish-green.

Colour Processing in LGN

After retinal processing, colour information is sent to the visual cortex via the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) in the thalamus. Different layers of the LGN process different aspects of visual input:

  • The parvocellular layers receive input from red and green cones and process wavelength (colour) and fine detail.

  • The koniocellular layers carry information from blue cones.

  • The magnocellular layers are mainly involved in processing motion and brightness, not colour.

This separation allows the brain to process different features of a visual scene in parallel.

Colour Processing the Extrastriate Cortex

The extrastriate cortex plays a crucial role in colour perception. Colour information from the parvocellular and koniocellular systems is sent along the ventral pathway to the inferior temporal lobe, which is responsible for processing object identity, including colour. The ventral pathway also receives input from the magnocellular system(mainly related to motion and brightness).

In contrast, the dorsal pathway primarily processes magnocellular information and is more involved in spatial awareness and motion than in colour processing.

Interestingly, it is possible to experience a loss of colour vision following a lesion to a specific region of the extrastriate cortex, without impacting visual acuity (as the photoreceptors are unaffected). This condition is called cerebral achromatopsia, and can even cause individuals to have difficulties imagining and remembering the colours of objects they once knew (before the brain lesion).

Form Perception

Studying individuals with visual agnosia has provided a lot of insight into form perception.

Visual agnosia is characterised by inability to identify common items by sight, although visual acuity remains; it is thought to be caused by injury to parts of the extrastriate cortex contributing to the ventral stream

A region of the extrastriate cortex called the Lateral Occipital Complex (LOC) appears to respond to a wide variety of objects and shapes

There also appears to be a few regions that primarily process specific categories - specialisation (faces, bodies, scenes - image on right)

Fusiform Face Area

  • Within the ventral stream, there are special face recognising circuits in the fusiform face area (FFA) which is a region of the visual association cortex located in the fusiform gyrus on the base of the temporal lobe. 

  • People can experience prosopagnosia following injury to the FFA (acquired prosopagnosia), or they can experience prosopagnosia from birth (congenital prosopagnosia). 

  • Finally, it is worth knowing that the fusiform face area has also been called the flexible face area. This name has arisen because the FFA has been implicated in expert object recognition more generally. For example, when bird and car experts view images of birds and cars respectively, activation in the FFA changes - this is not the case for non-experts (Gauthier et al., 2000; Tarr & Gauthier, 200; Xu, 2005).

Perception of Depth and Location

Depth Perception

Monocular Depth Perception

  • Depth can be perceived using monocular cues (one eye). 

  • For instance,  perspective, and the relative size of objects on the retina can be used to determine where objects are located.

Binocular Depth Perception

  • Depth perception is also informed by binocular (two eyes) information and binocular disparity (the small difference between the image on the retinas of both eyes).

Disparity sensitive neurons are found throughout the striate and extrastriate cortex, and in both the ventral and dorsal pathways.

  • Those found in the dorsal stream are involved in spatial perception and respond to large, extended visual surfaces

  • Those found in the ventral stream are involved in object perception and respond to the contours of 3D objects

The parietal lobe is also involved in the perception of spatial location and somatosensory perception.

  • It receives auditory, visual, somatosensory and vestibular information, and is important for object location memory and perception, as well as for controlling eye and limb movements

  • The dorsal visual stream terminates in the posterior parietal cortex and is involved in processing movement and location information.

Motion Perception

Extrastriate Cortex and Motion Perception

  • Area V5/MT of the extrastriate contains neurons that respond to movement

  • Receives input directly from the striate cortex and other areas of the extrastriate,

  • Also receives input from superior colliculus (involved in reflexes and eye movements)