Children globally pay attention to cultural events like Christmas and birthdays.
Individual families often have their own culturally specific practices for these events.
Children possess a cognitive toolkit of social learning mechanisms.
This toolkit facilitates quick and accurate acquisition of conventional behaviors.
It also maintains cultural diversity across different populations (Legare, 2017; Lewis & Laland, 2012; Tomasello, 2016).
Imitation serves as a social tool.
It satisfies social motivations.
Motivations include affiliating with in-group members and ensuring group status by adhering to group-specific norms (Nielsen, 2018; Over, 2020).
Children acquire knowledge from adults' testimony.
Studied using the selective trust paradigm.
Children choose between knowledge sources for new information (Tong et al., 2020).
Children filter and select reliable, trustworthy sources based on social markers (e.g., consensus, language) and epistemic characteristics (e.g., accuracy, competency; Harris et al., 2018).
Research fields evaluate the interplay between social and instrumental/epistemic factors.
Imitation and selective trust literatures offer different views and interpretations of children's social learning mechanisms.
These findings and views are complementary, not contradictory.
Imitation researchers view high-fidelity imitation as a social tool for affiliation (Nielsen & Blank, 2011) and norm adherence (Kenward, 2012; Keupp et al., 2013).
Selective trust researchers emphasize children's ability to determine the most trustworthy person to learn from (Harris & Corriveau, 2011; Sobel & Kushnir, 2013).
Children's imitative tendency may stem from selecting in-group members as ‘cultural experts’ with reliable group-specific knowledge, making their actions worth copying.
Imitation capacity emerges early in life (Barr et al., 1996; Meltzoff, 1985, 1988).
Infants develop the ability to identify intentions and goals of behaviors within their first 2 years.
They rapidly acquire specific means of reaching those goals (Casler & Kelemen, 2005; Gergely et al., 1995).
This involves learning and practicing instrumental skills like using utensils and assembling objects.
Young children increasingly acquire and re-enact social conventions with age (Vredenburgh et al., 2015), even if it means compromising efficiency (Yu & Kushnir, 2014).
Children faithfully copy an adult's arbitrary approach around age 2 (Nielsen, 2006).
Nielsen (2006) study:
Children shown how to open a box using an object to push a button, even though using a hand was simpler.
2-year-olds copied using the object, even when it led to task failure.
12-month-olds preferred using their hands.
18-month-olds imitated when the model was socially engaging (eye contact, responsiveness).
Social engagement didn't affect 2-year-olds; they were motivated to achieve the outcome and replicate the model's action when the model was socially active.
This highlights children's increasing tendency to replicate arbitrary information and the influence of social interaction in social learning.
Around 3–4 years, children replicate instrumentally relevant actions and faithfully replicate nonfunctional actions (e.g., swiping a box with a tool before opening it).
Older children replicate ritual-like actions not directly on a target object (Clay et al., 2018; Nielsen et al., 2018).
This is known as overimitation (Hoehl et al., 2019).
During preschool years, imitation shifts from goal-oriented to process-oriented, prioritizing faithful replication.
Gellén and Buttelmann (2019) found:
1-year-olds copied using their head to switch on a light box (when hands were free) if the model's hands were free, but not if the model hid her hands (replicating Gergely et al., 2002).
3-year-olds copied using their head regardless of the model's hand position.
Being less goal-oriented doesn't mean sacrificing the desired outcome.
Children replicate the ‘process’ even when the task has a different goal (Zhao et al., 2023).
Children followed the process of building a tower with pop sticks and marshmallows, regardless of whether the task was described as tower building or marshmallow collection.
Children in the marshmallow collection condition collected more treats.
These data indicate that children value ‘process’ regardless of its efficiency.
Imitation serves as a signal for social affiliation, conveying commitment to a group by behaving similarly (Nielsen & Blank, 2011).
Children overimitate, even though they can identify and skip irrelevant actions (Lyons et al., 2011) or verbally acknowledge that the actions are unnecessary (Kenward, 2012).
They perceive these actions as ‘normative rules’ (Kenward, 2012; Keupp et al., 2013) and enforce social norms on others (Schmidt, Hardecker, & Tomasello, 2016; Schmidt & Tomasello, 2012).
Prevailing view: high-fidelity imitation is driven by strong social motivations (Nielsen, 2018; Over, 2020; Over & Carpenter, 2012).
It's an inclination towards recognizing conventional rules and fulfilling normative expectations of behaving in equal ways.
Children show heightened imitation of models who resemble themselves (Wood et al., 2013).
Children use social markers to guide imitative decisions, determine from whom to learn, and identify in-group members.
They also use social markers to identify normatively accepted behaviors/preferences within their cultural in-group (Rakoczy & Schmidt, 2013).
Infants as young as 3 months show stronger preference for faces of their own race (Bar-Haim et al., 2006; Kelly et al., 2005).
Children use spoken language as a social category marker to guide decisions about whose information to acquire (Kinzler et al., 2010).
11-month-olds show enhanced brain activity when interacting with native speakers (Begus et al., 2016).
14-month-olds imitate actions by native language speakers more strongly (Buttelmann et al., 2013), also found in 3-year-olds (Howard et al., 2015).
Altınok et al. (2022) revealed the same effect with 18-month-olds.
Subtle differences in accent also likely affect social preference.
By 4 years, children prefer information from native-accented speakers (Kinzler et al., 2011).
Kinzler et al. (2011) study:
Children heard two models speak with native or foreign accents and then silently demonstrate object functions.
Children preferred learning from the native-accented model, regardless of the model's instructions or nonlinguistic information.
Identifying social conventions uniquely attributed to one's cultural group is essential for children's learning (Diesendruck & Markson, 2011).
Consensus is a straightforward cue indicating conventional behavior (Legare & Nielsen, 2015).
Can be induced verbally (e.g., ‘everyone always does it this way’) (Clegg & Legare, 2016) or by contrasting a majority with a minority (Wilks et al., 2015).
Nonverbal cues like nodding and head shaking guide children's decisions (Schillaci & Kelemen, 2014).
Fusaro and Harris (2008) study:
4-year-olds copied actions modeled by someone supported by a group through smiles and head nods, rather than someone who received frowns and head shakes.
Children rely on social markers to guide decisions about whose information and behavior to follow, perceiving it as socially approved cultural knowledge.
‘Selectivity’ is conceptualized differently in imitation and trust fields—as a social form of affiliation vs. an epistemic form of trusting a credible information source.
Integrating these concepts will enrich our understanding of children's social cognition.
By 4 years, children develop strong instrumental judgments and don't always copy others.
This cognitive skill resists potential pitfalls or erroneous information.
Selective trust literature focuses on recognizing epistemic characteristics (e.g., accuracy) that signal an informant's reliability (Pasquini et al., 2007).
It also involves selectively avoiding informants who have demonstrated inaccurate information or malicious intentions (Sperber et al., 2010).
Children prefer learning from previously accurate informants.
Meta-analytic findings:
When informants differed in epistemic and social characteristics, 3-year-olds weighed them equivalently.
4- to 6-year-olds prioritized epistemic cues (Tong et al., 2020).
Examples:
4-year-olds (but not 3-year-olds) are unlikely to trust a familiar teacher with a history of inaccurate decisions (Corriveau, Harris, et al., 2009).
They are likely to eschew labels suggested by an in-group member with low past accuracy (Corriveau, Kinzler, & Harris, 2013).
Although children prefer learning from a consensus, they may not if the information is questionable (Corriveau, Fusaro, & Harris, 2009).
Schillaci and Kelemen (2014) study:
Children watched a video of adults commenting on novel artifacts.
In ‘weak’ trials, the majority suggested an alternative but plausible function, while the minority suggested another plausible function (e.g., using a spongy tool to polish shoes vs. clean CDs).
In ‘strong’ trials, the majority suggested an implausible function (e.g., using a spiral egg cup to drink from) against the minority's plausible function (using it to hold an egg).
In weak trials, children showed no preference, but in strong trials, 4-year-olds avoided the implausible functions.
This was only the case for 4-year-olds but not for 3-year-olds.
This epistemic decision-making is consistent with findings in the imitation literature.
Wilks, Collier-Baker and Nielsen (2015) study:
Preschool children observed two methods for opening a puzzle box by a consensus group (majority) and an individual (minority).
Children were assigned to the majority group.
When both methods led to success, children copied the in-group way.
If the in-group method failed, they copied the lone individual.
DiYanni & Kelemen (2008) study of ineffective tool use:
Children presented with a goal (crushing a cookie) and observed a model rejecting a functional tool for a nonfunctional one.
Most children chose the functional tool, overlooking the model's cues.
This low imitation of an ineffective method has been replicated (Corriveau et al., 2017; DiYanni et al., 2015; Fong, Sommer, et al., 2021).
This pattern was less stable in 3-year-olds; emphasizing the socially preferred tool enhanced their choice of the suboptimal tool (DiYanni & Kelemen, 2008; Fong, Sommer, et al., 2021).
Understanding the intended goal and excluding flawed methods is essential for a functioning cultural group (Carpenter & Call, 2009).
3-year-olds' social learning is modulated by social cues, while 4 years appears to be a point at which children display rational judgment of whom to trust and what to learn.
Imitation and selective trust researchers study social learning using different theoretical frameworks and experimental paradigms.
Findings from both fields share analogically similar theoretical underpinnings and offer complementary explanations.
Children are driven to replicate others to satisfy social motivations, and selectively trust researchers depict children's preference for a social model as a reliable information source.
Children flexibly switch between social/conventional and instrumental/perceptual modes of social learning (Corriveau & Harris, 2010; Legare & Nielsen, 2015).
Children, viewed as young anthropologists (Harris, 2012), select reliable informants to acquire cultural knowledge.
Social models (who share characteristics with the learner) may be perceived as cultural experts with useful knowledge.
A social model may not always be a cultural expert, even if they share identities like nationality or gender.
Expertise in selective trust studies is manipulated based on domain-specific instrumental knowledge (Clegg et al., 2019; Koenig & Jaswal, 2011) rather than cultural knowledge.
Some researchers focus on social dominance as a trust inducer (Bernard et al., 2016; Margoni et al., 2022), while Henrich and Gil-White (2001) refer to prestige, learning from skilled individuals with cultural understanding.
Future research should investigate social informants as cultural experts delivering cultural knowledge.
Cultural environment factors (socialization goals, societal expectations; Keller et al., 2006) guide children's social learning.
Examples include parenting styles (Reifen Tagar et al., 2014), social dominance (Charafeddine et al., 2018; Fonn et al., 2022), temperament (Canfield et al., 2015), and attachment type (Corriveau, Fusaro, & Harris, 2009; Corriveau, Harris, et al., 2009).
These factors vary across diverse within- and between-population settings.
Cultural context shapes children's social learning decisions.
Studies show an impact of consensus and cultural group on imitation of ineffective tool use only within Asian-American children (Corriveau et al., 2017; DiYanni et al., 2015).
Indigenous Australian children used a more collaborative strategy (Nielsen et al., 2016).
ni-Vanuatu children replicated ritual-like actions (Clegg & Legare, 2016), and Indigenous Malaysian children replicate less efficient methods (Fong, Redshaw, & Nielsen, 2021).
The impact of social information on epistemic decisions may vary across cultural groups.
Researchers rely on demographic measures and ethnographies to interpret results and draw inferences.
More efforts are needed to develop measures that assess individual differences as cultural factors (Fong et al., 2022; Puttre & Corriveau, 2022).
The imitation literature extensively documents children's behavioral patterns in acquiring norms, focusing on what behaviors are replicated.
The selective trust literature provides insights into children's cognitive processes of selecting whose information to follow.
One explanation for social affiliation through imitation is that children trust these models as reliable sources of culturally-relevant information.
Allen et al. (2021) showed that children are less likely to overimitate when the demonstrator has a history of inaccuracy, and Rakoczy et al. (2009) showed that children protest against deviance from information endorsed by a reliable model.
Dragon and Poulin-Dubois (2023) failed to associate children's selective trust responses with their overimitation task performance.
Their selective trust task involved informants' accuracy, not social characteristics and their overimitation task evaluated imitative fidelity of conventional acts on common items demonstrated by a lone model via Zoom.
Future research should include selective trust tasks involving social decisions and novel overimitation tasks.
The imitation literature has research on children's developing imitative proclivities, and the selective trust literature offers evidence and theorization on children's developing epistemic vigilance.
Social biases guide children's decisions on whom to trust, and epistemic vigilance plays a role in selective imitation.
Combining the two approaches with investigations into individual differences and cultural factors will provide a holistic understanding of social learning mechanisms.
It will enable us to elucidate which aspects of social learning can be conceptualized using evolutionary explanations and what psychological processes may be shaped by cultural or environmental factors.
Future investigations will shed light on our flexibility and adaptability towards changing cultural or ecological demands, focusing on how selective trust and high-fidelity imitation may work in tandem.
Understanding of cultural evolution will be richer with efforts at more fully integrating different fields of social cognitive developmental research.
Overimitation is a subtype of high-fidelity imitation that involves replicating functionally irrelevant or ritual-like actions.
It differs from replicating arbitrary actions, less effective methods, or suboptimal methods because the actions are functionally relevant to reach the end goal.