Psych 204: Lecture Notes on Physical Attractiveness, Attraction, and Related Factors
Course context and reading guidance
- Welcome back, emphasis on in-person attendance to enhance learning.
- Reading guidance varies by lecturer; in this section, the instructor explains Talus-recommended readings:
- Readings on the Talus list reinforce content from lecture and are typically aligned with topics covered in class.
- If a reading is listed as related to a topic covered in class, it likely signals importance and potential assessment relevance.
- Not every Talus-listed reading will appear on a midterm/quiz, but many are considered important by the instructor.
- Reading timing:
- It is recommended to read after class to reinforce understanding and prepare for the midterm.
- Reading before class can still help, but the instructor emphasizes post-class review for deeper comprehension.
- Question about the Talus Reading List: clarifications provided, no further questions on the list at the moment.
- Today’s plan: finish discussing physical attractiveness and move toward broader attraction, with the goal of finishing that unit in the next two lectures.
- Administrative note: tomorrow (Thursday) will cover intimacy; Thursday marks the last topic lecture in the course.
Mere exposure effect: recap and scope
- Mere exposure effect explains attraction primarily at the initial stages of relationships.
- Important caveat discussed: simply knowing someone for a long time or having conflicts does not mean mere exposure always increases liking.
- The lecturer emphasizes that the mere exposure effect concerns initial-stage attraction, not long-term relationship dynamics.
- Takeaway: attraction processes discussed in class are focused on early relationship formation, not on later relationship development.
Big question: why are people attracted to physically attractive individuals?
- Core question is whether attraction to physical attractiveness is due to:
- Evolutionary arguments (reproductive fitness, health of offspring, signals of reproductive viability).
- Socialization and cultural norms (attractiveness as a valued trait reinforced by media, peers, and institutions).
- The discussion highlights that both genders value physical attractiveness, but perceptions of its importance can differ from actual behavior.
- The instructor notes a plan to revisit the “matching phenomenon” later in more detail (explicitly skipping this slide today to return to it with more depth).
Evolutionary argument for attractiveness
- Claim: physical attractiveness signals reproductive fitness and health, influencing mate choice as an indicator of potential offspring well-being.
- For men: attractiveness signals a good partner for reproduction.
- For women: attractiveness matters too, but resources are especially salient because they contribute to offspring welfare.
- The instructor remains cautious about fully endorsing this argument, acknowledging it is a perspective within evolutionary theory but not the sole explanation.
Socialization and cultural norms as alternative explanations
- Socialization argument: attractiveness is a stereotype that society teaches us to associate with positive traits.
- Core idea: “What is beautiful is good.”
- Sources of socialization include media, peers, family, education, and cultural institutions.
- The critique: evolutionary theory often has heteronormative implications; socialization theories provide critique by highlighting gender role biases and broader cultural conditioning.
- The instructor acknowledges the critique and notes that some evolutionary sub-pieces may allow for non-heteronormative viewpoints, but the mainstream lecture focuses on socialization as a powerful force shaping attractiveness perceptions.
Key studies and findings discussed
- Clifford & Walster (1973): socialization and attractiveness stereotype in educational settings
- Method: teachers were given identical information about a boy or girl; the only difference was a photograph showing an attractive or unattractive child.
- Outcome: attractive children were rated as more intelligent, more likely to succeed, and overall more positively, despite identical academic information.
- Conclusion: supports the physical attractiveness stereotype; attractiveness influences expectations even when all other data are identical.
- Disney and Cultivation Theory: media’s role in shaping attractiveness norms
- Cultivation Theory: frequent media consumption shapes beliefs and values embedded in media.
- Bazin et al. analyzed 21 animated Disney films and found that good/protagonist characters are almost always physically attractive and kind, while negative characters are often depicted as less physically appealing.
- Implication: media can socialize audiences to equate beauty with goodness and success, especially since Disney films are often consumed by children.
- Socialization of norms overall (Alice Eagley and colleagues, among others)
- Western cultural norms tend to associate attractiveness with happier, more sexually active, more outgoing, and more intelligent outcomes.
- Caveat: attractive appearance does not universally predict honesty or altruism; stereotypes have limits and do not apply to every trait or outcome.
- Media exposure and the contrast effect
- Exposure to highly attractive media can alter ratings of real people and self-perception through a contrast effect.
- Example: participants who watched media with attractive stars rated a target woman as less attractive than those who did not view the media.
- Mechanism: ongoing exposure to idealized beauty raises comparison standards; this can influence dating behavior and self-image in ways that may be disadvantageous or unrealistic.
- Perception of attractiveness and social skill (self-fulfilling prophecy)
- Studies where physically attractive individuals were perceived as more socially skilled, likable, and outgoing in phone calls (where appearance could not be seen).
- Interpretation: confidence and expectation biases (implicit egotism) may lead attractive people to act more socially adept; attractiveness can confer social advantages that reinforce these perceptions.
- Important caveat: capability is not determined by appearance; these effects reflect socialization and context, not determinism.
- Need fulfillment and beauty perception (Legat et al., 2013)
- Confederates and participant closeness influence perceived beauty.
- If a participant feels close to the confederate, they rate the confederate as more beautiful.
- Conclusion: interpersonal closeness can enhance perceived attractiveness, linking social connection to beauty judgments.
- Assortative mating: matching on attractiveness
- Concept: people tend to pair with others who have similar levels of attractiveness.
- Hunt et al. critique: assortative mating is strongest when couples do not know each other beforehand and decide based on appearance; if couples are friends before dating, attractiveness is less predictive of partner similarity.
- Data: 167 couples analyzed; correlation between partners’ attractiveness (r) is lower when couples were friends before dating, indicating friendship reduces the primacy of appearance in initial pairing.
- Interpretation: when people know each other before dating, other factors (shared interests, values, etc.) may play a larger role than appearance; otherwise, appearance similarity drives initial pairing.
- Summary takeaway on attractiveness studies
- Attractiveness interacts with target factors, situational factors, and perceiver factors in shaping attraction.
- Perceived similarity, similarity in traits, values, and interests tends to increase attraction (Birds of a feather).
- There is a potential for complementarity or self-expansion in some contexts, but similarity remains a robust predictor.
Perceiver factors in attraction
- Similarity vs. complementary dynamics
- Traditional view: similarity in traits, values, and interests predicts attraction.
- Evidence also supports some degree of complementarity (self-expansion) in certain contexts, but overall similarity is a stronger and more consistent predictor.
- Implicit egoism
- The tendency to prefer people who resemble ourselves (names, values, etc.) and to assume shared attributes.
- This mechanism can bias initial attraction toward those who resemble us, even if not consciously noted.
- Liking and attraction
- General principle: liking for us increases attraction to others; reciprocal liking enhances perceived attractiveness and closeness.
Target factors, situational factors, and perceiver factors (recap)
- Target factors: features of the person being evaluated (e.g., physical attractiveness, warmth, competence cues).
- Situational factors: context in which evaluation occurs (e.g., presence of media, social setting).
- Perceiver factors: individual differences in the observer (e.g., prior experiences, cultural background, implicit biases).
- Overall: attraction is a product of interaction among these three factors, making it complex and context-dependent.
Practical implications and ethical/philosophical considerations
- Media socialization effects can contribute to privilege for physically attractive individuals, influencing opportunities and social judgments.
- The “what is beautiful is good” stereotype is not universal; while common, it has limits and can be challenged by awareness of bias and context.
- Evolutionary explanations offer one lens but may overlook socialization, culture, and power dynamics (including gender norms, heteronormativity concerns).
- The contrast effect and media exposure highlight the pervasive influence of contemporary media ecosystems on dating standards and self-perception.
- Assortative mating findings suggest that friendships before dating can buffer the emphasis on appearance, pointing to the role of shared experience and compatibility beyond looks.
- Ethical questions arise about how to navigate attraction in a media-saturated environment and how to mitigate bias in educational and social settings.
- Correlation between partners’ attractiveness (assortative mating):
r = ext{Corr}(A1, A2)
where $A1$ and $A2$ are the attractiveness ratings of each partner. - Initial vs. post-exposure attractiveness (contrast effect):
Let $A{ ext{pre}}$ be the initial rating and $A{ ext{post}}$ the rating after exposure to attractive media. Then the change is:
riangle A = A{ ext{post}} - A{ ext{pre}}
typically $ riangle A < 0$ in contrast effect scenarios, indicating a lower post-exposure rating. - Need fulfillment and perceived beauty (conceptual relationship):
If we denote perceived beauty as $B$ and closeness as $C$, a simplified relation could be:
B = eta0 + eta1 C + ext{noise}
with $eta_1 > 0$, based on Legat et al. (2013) findings that increased closeness enhances perceived beauty. - Fitness signaling (evolutionary argument):
Attractiveness as a cue of reproductive fitness $F$ can be conceptualized as a positive association, though not expressed with a single universal equation in the lecture. The discussion notes that attractiveness correlates with perceived health and offspring viability in some evolutionary perspectives.
Summary of key takeaways
- Attractiveness influences initial attraction through multiple pathways, including socialization, media exposure, and perceived social benefits.
- The mere exposure effect is most relevant to initial attraction; it does not guarantee long-term relationship satisfaction or attraction.
- Evolutionary explanations offer one lens (reproductive fitness), but socialization and media influence provide robust, empirically supported mechanisms for how attractiveness is perceived and valued in society.
- The physical attractiveness stereotype is pervasive but not universal; its applicability varies across traits and contexts.
- Media exposure can shift both how we view others and how we view ourselves (contrast effect); the modern media landscape intensifies these effects due to constant exposure to highly attractive figures.
- Assortative mating shows that appearance-based pairing is stronger when dating decisions occur without prior knowledge of the other person; knowing someone beforehand attenuates the reliance on attractiveness.
- Perceiver factors like similarity, implicit egotism, and perceived liking contribute to attraction; long-term dynamics can shift attraction as relationships mature (e.g., from passionate to companionate love).
- The next class will cover intimacy, with a note that the last two lectures will complete the attraction/intimacy unit.
Connections to prior content and real-world relevance
- Links to foundational psychology concepts: stereotypes, social cognition, and biases in judgment.
- Real-world relevance: media literacy and critical assessment of beauty standards; understanding dating dynamics in the age of social media; recognizing privilege associated with attractiveness.
- Ethical considerations: acknowledging how stereotypes can affect opportunities in education and social life; discussing heteronormative biases in evolutionary theories and promoting inclusive perspectives.
Questions and takeaways for exam preparation
- Be able to describe the physical attractiveness stereotype and its limitations.
- Explain the difference between evolutionary arguments and socialization arguments for attractiveness.
- Identify and summarize key findings from Clifford & Walster (1973) and the Disney/Cultivation Theory line of work.
- Define the contrast effect and provide an example from media exposure research.
- Explain assortative mating and how friendship before dating changes the strength of attractiveness-based pairing.
- Describe Legat et al. (2013) findings on need fulfillment and perceived beauty.
- Distinguish target, situational, and perceiver factors in attraction, and discuss implicit egoism and similarity concepts.
- Prepare to discuss the role of intimacy in the final lectures and how attraction evolves over time in relationships.
Next class topics
- Finish discussion of attraction and begin intimacy research.
- Explore how attraction transitions from initial liking to deeper relational closeness, including the shift from passionate to companionate love.
- Revisit the mere exposure effect in the context of ongoing relationships and conflicts.